Social Psychology

Published on February 2017 | Categories: Documents | Downloads: 48 | Comments: 0 | Views: 690
of 336
Download PDF   Embed   Report

Comments

Content

Mm
mmm

'

r

'

.r-

'

.1

:

f

I

(

! ;

i

;,(

t'm

»

Vm

^Kn.n.T(

f».V» f'-Tl (•

--I

l!i:^^^y^r^^itil;^^^i4[^rf!i^^^d^'r^';rM;;|^;ri::;;,;:;

'I

H

'i

'"

Digitized by the Internet Archive
in

2011 with funding from

Boston Library Consortium IVIember Libraries

http://www.archive.org/details/socialpsychologyOOfarn

The Dryden

Press

Publications in Psychology

GENERAL EDITOR

THEODORE

M.

NEWCOMB

UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN

THE

SOCIAL

PSYCHOLOGY
OF

MUSIC
Paul R. Farnsworth
PROFESSOR OF PSYCHOLOGY
STANFORD UNIVERSITY

THE DRYDEN PRESS NEW YORK


316131

Copyright

©

igs^>fy ^^^ Dry den

All rights reserved.

The Drjden

Press, Inc.,

Press, Inc.

For information address

110 West 5yth

Street,

Library of Congress Catalogue Card

New

York ig, N.Y.

Number ^7—11^72

TO

Max

F.

Meyer

n'^ l/iW;^

Printed in the United States oj America

Preface

While

all

the arts have, from time to time, been subjected to

scientific scrutiny,
scientists.

music particularly has attracted the attention of

Musical instruments and their tonal potentialities have

traditionally

been the concern of the

physicists, but a

number of

Physiologists and physiological

psychologists share this interest.

psychologists have studied the auditory sense organs to learn the nature

of the neurological processes by means of vs^hich tones are perceived.

Laboratory psychologists have attempted to establish just what the

musician does

when he performs and

he possesses.

Still

to find

what

tonal sensitivities

other psychologists and a fe"w sociologists have

focused their interests on the affective and evaluative aspects of music.
All these interests are reflected to
that treat the area

some extent

where music and psychology

however, there has been no complete agreement

One

in the textbooks

overlap.
as to

So

far,

what are the

may stress laboratory data and do a minimum of theorizing, another may present fewer
"facts" but be far more philosophical in approach. One may treat of

boundaries of the psychology of music.

music and musicians
another

music

as if the cultural

milieu were of little importance,

may show more concern with

the social determination of

activities.

Of course no book
been

text

said to

can include

all

that at

one time or another has

be subsumed under the psychology of music.

All that

PREFACE
can properly be asked of an author
as

a coherent picture of the

is

that

he present what he regards

phenomena of the

field,

major

its

problems, and the solutions he deems most plausible. This, then,

is

what the present book attempts to do.

The psychology of music
books issued

in the past

has been the subject of at least six text-

two decades. The year 1937 saw the publicaA Psychology of Music and Mursell's The

tion of Diserens and Fine's
Vsjchology of Music.

The former book not only

dealt with experimental

literature but also devoted considerable space to the origin of music,

animal auditors, mythology and folklore, and the relation of music to

magic and sorcery and to religion, melancholy, and
Mursell book philosophized

much more and

interpreted research

data rather narrowly in terms of Gestalt theory.

music teachers was reserved for Music and

The

ecstasy.

Mursell's advice to

the Classroom Teacher

and

other treatises.
Seashore's Psychology of Music was issued in 1938,

With no interest in

those aspects of music which are primarily social psychological Seashore
,

limited himself largely to a description of the excellent but apparatus-

bound

studies of his

broaden

later to

In Search

his

own

laboratories.

coverage of the

of Beauty in Music.

field

He attempted some

years

with Why We Love Music and

Seashore prided himself on following no

"school" of psychology. Yet he was one of the most ardent hereditarians

psychology has produced, and his books quite clearly reflect

this nativistic bias.

In 1940

one of Seashore's students.

The Psychology of Music.
Music,

Built

on

Max

his earlier

Beautiful in

Schoen' s book presented a well-rounded picture of the

experimental findings through the 1930's.
sophical

work

No new
known

Schoen, published his

work, The

A

later,

more

of Schoen's appeared as The Understanding of Music.

texts appeared after 1940 until 195^3,

Eirfiihrung in die Musikpsjchologie

when

Revesz's well-

was translated into English

and published in Great Britain in somewhat revised form.
vi

philo-

A

year

PREFACE
later

it

was issued

in the

in

title Introduction to

Revesz's book contained a good survey of the

the Psychology of Music.

European findings

United States under the

music psychology but touched only

American research. The tone of the Revesz volume was

lightly

on

absolutistic

and hereditarian.
Lundin's An Objective Tsychology of Music was also published in 19^3.

As the only American text to appear

in thirteen years,

it

had the

important task of digesting the research materials of more than a
decade. This book has a cultural and relativistic flavor and an interbehavioristic orientation.

Two

other American books have influenced music-psychology

research, although they are not formal texts in the psychology of

music.

One was

Pratt's The Meaning of Music,

essays rather than a

which was

a series of

textbook and hence made no attempt to cover

the experimental literature. Showing a high degree of sophistication,
Pratt's little

book was written

in large part as a defense of formalism.

The other work was given the
Arithmetic.

Its

author,

is

the

Indeed,

field.

first

Max Meyer,

one of the most

dedicated,

it

rather frightening
to

whom

brilliant theorists

title

The Musician

s

book

is

and experimenters

in

the present

was the manuscript of Meyer's

treatise

which

stirred the interest of this author in the psychology of music,

Meyer's book offered exciting neurological speculations
important research data, but

it

is

admittedly

difficult to

therefore has had an extremely limited audience.
years the present writer has

become

as

well

as

read and

Although over the

increasingly disappointed at the

slow growth of scientific neurology and has occupied himself less and
less

with theorizing along the

lines suggested

by Meyer, he

still

appreciates Meyer's refreshing approach and feels that the serious

student will find The Musician
his

s

Arithmetic,

more recent How We Hear well worth

Meyer's early

and

careful attention.

During the three decades since the author
works, he has been occupied with

articles,

first

came upon Meyer's

a host of research

problems and has
vii

PREFACE
finally

adopted a

relativistic, culturally

would be one of the
to

first

answer the problems of music

course,

may never be

least tentative

He

oriented point of view.

to admit that scientists have hardly

begun

Many problems,
good number already have

aesthetics.

resolved. Yet a

of
at

answers, the elaboration of which will be found in the
Since

chapters to follow.

little

experimental

done on non-European music and the

work

less serious

has so far been

forms of Occidental

music, this book will necessarily be concerned mainly with the
serious music of the West.

To write
task.

Yet

both psychological and musical audiences

for

book attempts

this

to interest these

that

which any

educated adult usually possesses.

intelligent

more

psychological

But unless the reader has some

beyond the

title

His musical knowledge, however, need not be exten-

of this book.

To

It is

and reasonably well-

sustained interest in music he probably will not look

sive.

a difficult

two audiences.

the author's belief that the reader will need no

knowledge than

is

help him with the psychological and musical terms with

which he may be unfamiliar,
For the reader

who

a glossary

more

desires

is

detailed

provided on pp. 280—287.

knowledge
o of the research

material described in the articles listed in the footnotes, a key to

the reference abbreviations

The reader who wishes

is

to

given on pp. 288—292.

keep abreast of the work being done in

psychological aesthetics should consult the Journal of Aesthetics and

which once

Art Criticism,

a year carries a list of articles that have

appeared during the previous calendar year.
articles

which

interest

him and before reading the

After noting the
articles themselves,

the reader would be advised to examine the abstracts printed six

times a year in Psjchological

Abstracts.

coverage of the older articles than
is

referred to

Allied Fields

volumes
Vlll

i

A

is

For a more comprehensive

offered by this book, the reader

Bibliography of Periodical Literature in Musicology and

and a Record of Graduate Theses Accepted (published

and

2

in

as

1940 and 1943 by the American Council of

PREFACE
Attention should also be directed to A. R.

Learned Societies).

Chandler, "A Bibliography of Experimental Aesthetics, 1865"— 1932"
(Ohio State
E.

N.

University

Barnhart,

Aesthetics,

A

Studies,

1

[1933]);

Bibliography

to

A.

R.

of Psychological

Chandler and

and Experimental

1864—IP3J (Berkeley, University of California Press,
W. A. Hammond, A Bibliographj of Aesthetics and of the

1938); and to

Philosophy of the Fine Arts from

igoo

to

1932 (New York, Longmans,

Green, 1934)-

The author

is

indebted to

Since in general they
here.

An

know

many people and

name them

exception must be made, however, in the case of the

Stanford University Press, which kindly allowed
for this

publishing houses.

of his appreciation he will not

book almost the

This material

him

to take over

entire manuscript of his earlier Musical Taste.

now makes up much

of Chapters 6 and

7.

P.R.F.
Stanford, California

July 1957

IX

Contents

Chapter One

The Psychological Approach

Music

to

i

The Psychology of Music and the Social Sciences, 2
The Unreliability of Common Sense, 3 The Search for




Alternative Hypotheses, 4

The Possibilities of Research
Experimental Aesthetics, 7
The Absence of

musical Investigations, 6
in

Limitations of Psycho-







Absolutes,

I

2



A

Preview of Later Chapters,

i

3

Chapter Two

The Social Psychology oj Musical Scales
The Tone Elements of Music,
Times,

1

8

17



Scales of Ancient

Scale of Just Intonation,



Number

17

2

i



A

Scale w^ith

The Need for Modulation,
Mean-Tone Temperament, 24 Equal Tempera23
ment, 2^
The Stability of Intervals, 26
Equal
Temperament, Our Frame of Reference, 28 Other
Possible Scales, 29
Summary, 31
the Prime

7, 23







'







Chapter Three

The Interval

36

Distinctive Quale, 36

37





Vibrato, Tremolo, and Trill,

Apparent Pitch of

Minor

Effect,

lutions,

44

sonance, 47





40



Intervals,

38

Finality Effects, 41

Tonality,

4^







The Major-

Interval Reso-

Consonance and Dis-

Summary, £o
XI

CONTENTS
Chapter Four

Melody

gg

Melody and
Melody and Loudness, 6i Melody
and Timbre, 64 Melody and Sonance, 66 Melody
and Noise, 67 Melody and Tempo, 69 Melody and
Rhythm, 70 Harmony, j £ Summary, 77
Principles of Attention and Learning, gy

Pitch Level, ^8



















Chapter Five

Language Aspects of Music

84

Grammar, 8g
Desire for Communication, 84
Alleged Key Effects, 86
Major and
Meaning, 8g
Color-Tone Linkage, 90
Minor Modes, 88
"The
Adjective Lists for
Language of the Emotions," 93
Classifying Music, 9^ Variables Which Give Meaning
to Music, 99
The Expression of Tensions, 102
Music as a Universal Language, 106 Psychoanalytic
Symbolism, 108 Summary, 109
























Chapter Six

The Nature oj Musical Taste

116

Whimsey or Law? 117' Eminence, 120

Enjoyment,
129- Know^ledge of Composers, 1 3 1 Programs, 132*
Individual and Group
Space Allocations, 136
Differences, 138
Criteria and Conditioners of








Taste, 142



Summary, 1^2

Chapter Seven

The Measures oJ Musical Taste

A

Variety of Measures,

i

^8



i^'S

Auditory Tests,

1

5^9



Paper-and-Pencil Tests, 16^ Polling, 167 Orchestral
Programs, 167
Broadcasts of Recordings, 168








Record Listings, 169 Scholarly Texts, 170
dom, 173 Summary, 174




xii



Bore-

CONTENTS
Chapter Eight

The Nature of Musical Abilities
Ability

—an

178

Appropriate Descriptive Term,

178



Ability in Music and the
Generality of Ability, 179
Other Arts, 182 Academic Intelligence and Musical




The

Hereditability of Musical Abilities,
and Body Structures, 188
Adlerian
Views on Ability, 192' Jungian Views on Ability, 193Freudian Views on Ability, 1 94 Imagery as a Source of
Developing Abilities, 196 Training
Abilities, 195"
Methods: General Problems, 199- Training Methods
Special Problems, 204* Creativity, 207' Summary, 212
Ability, 183

184





Abilities









Chapter Nine

226

The Measurement of Musical Abilities
Tests of Verbal Knowledge, 226

Musical
231



Skills,

230

Tests of Nonverbal



Unstandardized Aptitude Tests,



The Original Seashore

Gretsch Test for Musical

Battery,

235^

Aptitude,

Tilson-



237
1939
Edition of the Seashore Measures of Musical Talents,


Kwalwasser-Dykema Music Tests, 240 Kwal237
wasser Music Talent Tests, 242
The Drake Tests,






Whistler-Thorpe Musical Aptitude Test, 244
Lundin Tests, 24^
Wing Standardized Tests of
243







Musical Intelligence, 246
Test, 247





Strong Vocational Interest

The Future of Music Aptitude

Tests, 248

Chapter Ten

Applications oj Music to Therapy and Industry

2^4

Physiological Changes, 2^4
The Present Status of
Music Therapy, 2^8 Music in Physical Therapy, 25-9
Music in Mental Therapy, 2 5^9 The Effects of Music
on Achievement, 264








Xlll

CONTENTS
Epilogue

273

Appendix

lj£

1.

2.

The Musical Taste of an American Elite, 275^
Glossary, 278
3. Key to Reference Abbrevi•



ations,

XIV

286

Subject Index

291

Name

299

Index

THE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
OF MUSIC

CHAPTER ONE

The Psychological Approach
to

JLiKE most

discussions

area of study

is

which attempt

show

arguments about the

a science,

psychology of music can settle

would allow science

to

that

all

particular

scientific status of the

Some

or nothing.

little

to include almost

some

Music

definitions

systematic investigation,

while others would restrict the term to the older and betterestablished disciplines of the natural sciences. Yet
that

the

psychologists

aesthetics of

who

music aim to be

have

interested

scientists.

They

it

can be truly said

themselves

try to

in

the

employ the best

tools available, to handle their experimental variables according to the

accepted rules of science, and to treat their findings with a reasonable
degree of
times

statistical

when

Admittedly, there have been

sophistication.

they have kept too close to their laboratories and have

withdrawn somewhat from musical

reality.

Several decades ago, for

example, a number of psychologists claimed to be able to

test innate

musical capacities. They also maintained that these tested capacities

could not be improved by training, a dogma which was most
tating to those musicians

ment

who had

devoted their

lives to the

irri-

improve-

knew that their labors had
more and more data have been collected,
have come to agree with the musicians that the

of tonal perception and control and

not been in vain.
the psychologists

But, as

"musical ear" can indeed be improved
logical aestheticians are

more

(p. i86).

careful to

work

Nowadays, psycho-

in a musical context.

THE PSYCHOLOGICAL APPROACH TO MUSIC
The Psychology of Music and the Social Sciences
It is

often said that psychology was born from a union of philosophy

and the natural sciences. With the passage of time, however, the
social sciences

began to envelop psychology

until, at present,

many

of the research problems and techniques of cultural anthropology,
sociology, and psychology are surprisingly alike.



psychology
sciences

its

retention of

its

not with philosophy) and the addition of

(if

interests in the social science area

of the psychology of music.

members of

This growth of

older affiliations with the natural



is

reflected in the

its

newer

development

Past workers in the field,

notably

the Carl Seashore group at the State University of Iowa,

rather limited themselves to experiments of the natural science sort
(p. 8).

Others, particularly researchers of the past few years, have

tended to be

at least equally interested in

techniques of the social sciences.

It is

problems adapted to the

natural that those

who

deal

primarily with the biological aspects of music should feel that the

more important antecedents of musical activity are to be found in the
nature of the human organism, whereas those further removed from
the natural science laboratory should look more to cultural forces for
their explanations.

While the present

treatise attempts to cover the

major experi-

ments of psychological aesthetics whatever the techniques employed,
its

orientation

is

admittedly a sociopsychological one.

Believing as

he does that the earlier workers tended to overstress the importance
of the biological bases of musical behavior almost to the point of

ignoring

its

cultural determinants, the author has here tried to bring

the picture a
to

some

to

little

more

to balance.

No

be an overbalance on the cultural

the effect, the choice has been deliberate.

become

doubt the result will seem

clearer as the

book

is

read.

Its

side.

But whatever

justification will perhaps

THE PSYCHOLOGICAL APPROACH TO MUSIC
The Unreliability of Common Sense
The behavioral
that he perceives.

obvious,

v^^hat

scientist tends to question the validity of

He

than the layman to accept the

less likely

is

much

"common

the dictionary terms

sense," for

he has

found that he and others "know" many things that are simply not
true.

Let us take

as

an illustration the very widespread belief that

marijuana heightens the auditory capacities.
addicts are convinced that they are

when

more

Even many musician-

sensitive to tonal stimuli

given this drug. Yet psychophysiological research gives no

support to the idea.^ In this area, alertness and the feeling of alertness are not closely related.

Or
more

let us

consider the almost universal belief that Negroes are

sensitive to tone

and rhythm than are whites.

Large numbers

of these two American subgroups have been tested in tonal

and

in the discrimination of differences in pitch,

rhythm, and timbre.

No

striking

memory

loudness, time,

Negro (or white) superiority has

ever been demonstrated. In the several studies in this area the racial
differences that do

emerge are

so slight that

one group or another will

yield the higher average score because of factors local to the

But

diate testing situation (Chap. 8).

of these subgroups were at
local conditions

Common

and make

if

the superiority of either one

impressive,

all

itself

sense teaches that

imme-

it

would transcend these

unmistakably apparent.^

some humans

are born monotones.

These unfortunates cannot carry tunes and are doomed to go through
life

embarrassed by the

fact that they

recognize our national anthem
political witch-hunting.



cannot sing or perhaps even

a serious disability in these days of

The follower of common

sense typically

accepts the fatalistic genetics of earlier decades and does nothing

about the matter. To him the monotone appears to have an inherent
anatomical defect.
educators

But luckily there are psychologists and music

who check on common

sense.

They have shown

that most,

THE PSYCHOLOGICAL APPROACH TO MUSIC
if

not

all,

abilities

monotones have psychological rather than

biological dis-

and can be helped to overcome their tune weakness. Mono-

tones are often people

who

the early grades of school. 3

have met with emotional
Possessed of

difficulties in

more than average

dislike

of the schoolroom or of the music teacher, or of both, and quite
frequently holding to the attitude that singing

is

an activity for

monotones may become uncooperative during

when music fundamentals
life

are usually

most

that

sissies,

early period

easily learned.

Later in

they will find that with great effort they can master the concept

of pitch but that they must have considerable aid from psychologists

or music educators to do so (p. i86).

Although many other
unreliability of

more example.

common

illustrations

could be given of the occasional

sense, let us content ourselves

with just one

Capacities for handling two-four, three-four, four-

measure seem to be

four, six-four, and even eight-four beats per

generally regarded as "instinctive."

American music students handle

such time signatures without formal training but flounder
to beat out the five-four or the seven-four.
"instinct" for these latter, says

common

Man

sense.

when asked

simply lacks the

Yet

if this

were

the lack should be general and not limited to a few culture areas.
psychologist

Max Meyer should not have found natives

so,

The

of North Africa

dancing and swaying to these beats. But, according to his observations,
certain of the natives

four as
his

we are with

were almost

with the

as skillful

the two- and three-four.

American laboratory, he found

it

Later,

five-

on

and seven-

his return to

quite easy to train his students

to tap out the five- and seven-four with great accuracy. 4 These

Americans soon forgot that they had no "instinct" for such

young

activities.

The Search Jor Alternative Hypotheses
Another characteristic of the
for rival hypotheses.

scientist

is

his willingness to search

While the layman may be content with one

THE PSYCHOLOGICAL APPROACH TO MUSIC
apparently reasonable "cause" for soine phenomenon, the scientist
investigates a variety of possible antecedents.

It

looks reasonable,

for example, to assume that the true beat in music5 has a rate
is

determined by some organic rhythm

many

like the heartbeat,

years the assumption of an intimate connection

function and music response was treated

matter to

test in

which

and for

between body

But Lund put the

as a fact.

an attempt to see whether there might not be some

other deterininants.^ Although he did not find what these latter were,

he did show that true beat and heartbeat have

During 1939

many performances

of the

Roth Quartet and of

orchestras and found the true beat to vary



association.

psychologist took meticulous measureinents of

this

100 pulsations

little

a far cry

all

several

the

symphony

way from 40

from the 70—75" beats the heart

to

gives each

minute.

The search
accept

as

creating
that
if it

a

for alternative hypotheses

finally valid all that gets

new

makes the

into print.

scientist loath to

In the process of

theories and rechecking behavior he sometimes finds

what the books

say about

some phenomenon

is

no longer true

ever was. Thus, long ago the notion was written into at least

few textbooks

that the great vocalists sang pure,

Happily, this invalid observation appears in print
fact, it is a

tones.

but rarely. In

Avonder that anyone ever did hold to the purity idea, for

one needs only to

listen to a relatively

fork, to realize that

music

now

steady

deals.

And

it is

there

pure tone, say that of a tuning

the impure and not the pure tone with which
is a

host of excellent psychological works on

the vibrato which demonstrate beyond doubt the unsteadiness of the

preferred vocal and violin tone (p. 8).

Other almost

totally incorrect sets of musical rules can

be found

some of the older books on baton movements. Typical diagrams
show a preponderance of straight-line motions and periods of

in

relative

rest

at

the

beat-instants.

Bartholomew checked these

diagrams against the baton performances of conductors. 7

He attached

THE PSYCHOLOGICAL APPROACH TO MUSIC
lamp to the end of

a tiny
light

dimmed

a

baton which had been so wired that the

at the exact instant of the beat.

By photographing the

path of the light this experimenter found curvilinear rather than
straight-line

motions and points of

The

respective beats.

fastest,

rest closer to the "ands" after the

not the slowest, speeds were often

at

the beat-instants

On

misinformation

printed

occasion,

practical reasons, the ethical aspects of

As an

illustration let us take

been

has

retained

for

which are not of a high order.

one of the author's minor

studies.

Around 1930 considerable research was being undertaken with a
make of player piano. Its manufacturer had long been

certain

advertising that

it

offered sixteen different intensities of

strokes, roughly twice the loudness possibilities of

two chief

This statement was true enough from the standpoint of physics

rivals.

as

its

hammer

many

there actually were twice as

being studied. But,

as the

on the music

intensity holes

roll

author clearly proved, only one half of

its

loudness differences were discriminable by the typical layman whereas
all

of

its rivals'

were. Musically and practically speaking, then, the

competing instruments were equally good
ness differences. Yet the advertising

in their handling of loud-

went on unchanged.

Limitations of Psjchomusical Investigations

The psychologist
organized whole.

pieces

But why

together bits of information into an

does he choose to

problems and not on others? His choice,

it

work on

would seem,

is

certain
to

some

extent forced by the availability and cooperation of his subjects and

by the degree to which the complex phenomena of music can be
subjected to scientific analysis.

The

first

of these conditioners

research choices of the psychologist.

human
6

is

common

Unless he

is

to

almost

all

the

working with sub-

animals which can be caged, or with the semicaged school

THE PSYCHOLOGICAL APPROACH TO MUSIC
child or college sophomore,

who must

cooperate willy-nilly, the

psychologist often has great selection and motivation difficulties with
his subjects.

He commonly

he might desire to

answer

test,

his questions

cannot get the cooperation of

and even those

without

who do

all

those

"cooperate"

may

His findings, then, are

sufficient care.

not always truly representative.

The second

factor

which

limits psychomusical research operates

throughout the social sciences and the humanities.
are rarely simple, and

whenever

analyses are to

Causal relations

be made care must

be taken that the dynamic interrelation of the phenomena
is

be devoid of musical meaning. The practical
is

in question

not disturbed. Otherwise, the data resulting from the analyses will

many

that

fascinating musical

effect of this limitation

problems cannot be studied by the

aid of any of the analytic techniques currently used by the psycho-

In

logist.

He

many

areas

he can only chew along the edges, so to speak.

cannot get his teeth into the meaty center of the problem.

The

Psychological

what

of Research in Experimental Aesthetics
research in aesthetics can often throw much light on

Possibilities

is

taking place in a particular music area, i.e.,

function.

it

has a descriptive

Research may sometimes yield, in addition, pertinent

information on the reasons for some particular bit of music behavior.
It

can also be of considerable aid in forecasting

criteria

for

all

work

by which

artistic

But no science

.

offers

responses can be rated as "good," "proper

time," "bad," or "improper for

all

time." In other words, the

of the psychological aesthetician leads to descriptions, expla-

nations, and forecasts, but does not reveal aesthetic absolutes.

THE DESCRIPTIVE FUNCTION.

To

illustrate

descriptive, function of psychological aesthetics

the

first,

or

we might well return

to a consideration of vocal and violin vibrato.^

Careful research in

THE PSYCHOLOGICAL APPROACH TO MUSIC
this area has

demonstrated that the cultivated singing voice of the

adult show^s periodic changes in pitch in approximately 9 ^ per cent of its


tones. Regular changes in intensity and timbre have also

been found

to occur. For both violin and voice the tone pulsates about 6* g times
a second.

The extent of the pulsation of the violin tone

pitch range)

approximately a quarter of a tone.

is

the vibrato extent

is

twice

this value

(in the

middle

For the vocal tone

although the listener can rarely

believe this and interprets the range as

somewhat smaller

(a fifth

of a

tone). Both the musically trained and the relatively unmusical prefer

the current vibrato rates to

all

others.

Untrained individuals prefer

a pitch range of approximately a quarter-tone, while the musically

trained favor a pitch

The

wobble of about

typical vibratos of a

measured. Hence,

it is

number of

now

whole tone.

a tenth of a

virtuosos have been carefully

possible for the aspiring

violinist to coinpare his vibrato

with that of

his

before some instrument which transmutes his

young singer or

model by performing

own

tonal efforts into

visual stimuli.

Vibrato of Some Well-known Singers*

* C.

Average Extent in

per Second

Whole Tones

Schumann- Heink

7-6

•38

Galli-Curci

7-3

•44

Caruso

7-1

•47

Rethberg

7-0

•49

Ponselle

6-9

•48

Chaliapin

6-8

•54

Jeritza

6-8

•53

Tetrazzinl

6-8

•37

Talley

6-7

•54

Tibbett

6-6

•55

Gigli

6-5

•57

Hackett

5-9

•47

Homer

5-9

•51

E. Seashore,

mission).

Average Rate

Psychology of Music, N.Y.,

McGraw-Hill, 1938,

p.

43 (with per-

THE PSYCHOLOGICAL APPROACH TO MUSIC
THE CAUSAL FUNCTION. ^°

While the second,

or

causal,

function of psychological aesthetics can be illustrated from any one

of a large

number

of only two, the

of studies, let us limit ourselves to a consideration

first

to be

concerned with the determiners of tempo

preferences and the second with the reasons for the high regard in

which the old Cremona-made

Tempo

violins are held.

preferences have been found to vary considerably from

person to person. This large range
factors,

to

but

is

no doubt due

to a

number

of

one of these factors has been isolated and found

at least

be what might be termed "occupational tempo." Thus Foley found

that girls studying trades like dressmaking in
at a

which

activity

proceeds

slow pace were prone to favor andante tempos; those working

with power machines,

slow allegro. Typists, with their

a

working speeds, tended to prefer
Their work speeds,

came

seems

it

a fast allegro

clear, so

faster

bordering on presto.

conditioned these

to prefer these rates even outside the shop

girls that

they

and the office."

And now for our second illustration of psychology's causal function. With the passage of time, the sales values of the violins built by
the old masters of Cremona have grown enormously. The know-how
of making great instruments has been lost and
are

weak

imitations,

Stradivarius or
its

some

it

modem

reproductions

commonly said.^^ But what makes a
Cremona fiddle so magnificent? Is it
Or may it not be, in part at least, a matter

is

other old

physical construction?

of attitude, of prestige long associated with this period of alleged

violin-making supremacy?

The

physical qualities of the old instruments and of their

imitations have been carefully examined by Saunders. ^3
prising finding

is

that

instruments differed

other old violins.

on

from

all
its

but one of his

modern copy

to

less

make an

His sur-

any one of the old

than

All the instruments, old and

alike in the response curves they yielded.

work required

tests

modem

it

did from the

new, were much

Saunders did find that the

old violin speak properly was, on the

THE PSYCHOLOGICAL APPROACH TO MUSIC
newer models. The

average, a little less than that needed for the

Cremona

violins

with their older

that are not quite so easily

wood and

drowned out by the other instruments of

But aside from

the orchestra. ^4

drier varnish yield tones

this

energy variable no great

differences emerged.

There

particular instrument
feel that the

tone

is

than he would for a

was made by

a great

modern

to pay

more

for

it

All that

is

needed to prove that

is

to arrange a psycho-

experiment in which Strads and their well-built inodern

logical

imitations are played behind a screen a

The

results

marvelous

violins.

number of times

in

prove that the person has yet to be found

consistently pick the Strad.^5

tions

him

validity

violin.

some

that a

master makes the listener

superior and stimulates

the suggestion hypothesis has

order.

The awareness

then, the suggestion hypothesis.

is,

The old masters,

it

random

who

would seem,

can

built

But along with the instruments they built reputa-

which w^ere even more marvelous

THE FORECASTING FUNCTION.

To

illustrate the third,

or

forecasting, function of psychological aesthetics let us look again to

the psychology of suggestion and next to the forecasting of grades in
conservatories of music.

suggestion
affected

we might

From what we know

forecast that musical preferences

by an experimenter

subjects' likes

and

dislikes.

shown by Rigg, when he
college students.

^^

of the principles of

if

he

set himself the task of altering his

That such

effects

can actually occur was

offered the saine music to three groups of

The members of one group were

what they were hearing

could be

in a romantic

light.

No

led to regard

special psychological

"atmosphere" was suggested to the members of the second group.

The

last

group of students was successfully led to associate the music

with Hitler and the Nazi movement. The proof that the three
different "atmospheres" elicited three different degrees of acceptance

was shown
lo

in the three

mean preference

scores.

When

thought of as

THE PSYCHOLOGICAL APPROACH TO MUSIC
Nazi music the compositions were least appreciated.

When

romantic atmosphere was suggested the acceptance was

greatest.

Knowing very well

that to

many laymen

the

word

the

"classical"

suggests high-brow, boring music, the arrangers of a Danish broadcast

program of serious music changed the

"Classical

of their series from

title

Music" to "Popular Music" but kept unchanged the

their musical offerings.

style of

The latter label, they felt, suggested pleasanter,

easier-to-grasp music. That they had properly gauged the connota-

was rather dramatically demonstrated

tions of these terms

that the
It

number of listeners doubled

after the

changed

in the fact

titling. ^7

can safely be predicted that certain compositions will be better

liked

if

the listener

is

led to think they

some eminence. Thus,
audiences

who

if

believe the

composer

Buxtehude, the acceptance will be
that the
effects

composer

is

were composed by

Bach's Concerto
to

far less

in

D

Minor

is

a

man

of

played to lay

be the relatively unknown
than

if

the listeners are told

their revered J, S. Bach,^^

Similar suggestive

have been demonstrated in the area of jazz preferences and in

the pictorial

arts.

And now let us look at the forecasting of conservatory grades.
Many colleges nowadays have established what they call "critical
levels" of college aptitude, minimum scores which an applicant must
reach in order to matriculate.

These

critical

levels

empirically determined from the scores of past failures.

have been

They

then, of considerable value for forecasting, since persons
scores

below these

critical points will

time of graduation.
Music, found a

almost certainly

fail

are,

making

before the

Stanton, working at the Eastman School of

critical level

She based her level on a

of this type for entering music students. ^9
test of tonal

imagery, a case history, a

college aptitude test, and a battery of music aptitude tests (Chap. 9),

After years of experimentation, Stanton found she could foretell

with considerable accuracy which of the applicants would be the
failures.
1

THE PSYCHOLOGICAL APPROACH TO MUSIC
The Absence of Absolutes
Because there are certain biological periodicities close to the 6 or
6'^ pulsations per second that

musician prefers above

the vibrato rate the contemporary

is

other rates, one theorist has assumed this

all

periodicity to be the "proper vibrato rate for

all

time."

To him

the fact that the musically elite currently prefer a rate identical with

one of the periodicities of "nature" proves that they

like v\^hat

it is

biologically proper to like.

This type of reasoning

he knows

that

what

is

is

not acceptable to the social

deemed proper

in

scientist, for

one period of time may not

be so considered in the next.^° The position taken

in this

book, the

belief that scientific research does not yield absolutes and final ansvv^ers,
is

well expressed by Tiffin in an article in which he describes some of

his vibrato researches:

This

work

intended to present objective unequivocal

is

information about the vibrato used by this generation of

and students of voice.
vibrato

is

included in the results.

by present day

improved
is

not contended that

is

if

It

it

were made

repeated

may

No

esthetic value

may be araued

artists is a fad

eliminated. This

study

It

ultimately beautiful.

type of

judgment

is

that the vibrato in use

and that their voices would be

less

prominent or even entirely

quite possibly be true.

fifty

this

artists

Perhaps

if this

years hence, the average extent of the

vibrato then in use will be found to be quite different from the

one

now employed.

artistry

have changed,

time to time.
taste

This will simply

nor to

.

.

.No

justify

as all esthetic

atteiTipt is

made

mean

that standards of

preferences change from
to prophesy future artistic

current preference in terms of ultimate

esthetic principles. ^^

Absolutes are not revealed by psychological research for the simple
reason that there are no musical absolutes to be found. There
12

is,

for

THE PSYCHOLOGICAL APPROACH TO MUSIC
example, no absolutely "good" music, music whose goodness transcends time and space. As the British psychologist Vernon phrases

"That music

it:

'good' which happens to appeal especially to the

is

subjective tastes of the musicians of the period, these tastes being to
a large extent

determined irrationally by temperamental and various

environmental conditions, by suggestion, contra-suggestion, conservatism and iconoclasticism."^^

A

Preview of Later Chapters
Our first consideration will be the

agreed upon placements of
or sing. The interval,

all

scale, the relative

and socially

the notes the musician attempts to play

any two simultaneously or successively

i.e.,

played tones, and the melody, a succession of intervals

felt to possess

unity, furnish the basic underpinninp^s of musical structure and are

for this reason considered in Chapters

Since the

first,

a piece of music

and

4.

and often the only, question many laymen ask about

is,

"What

story does the composition tell?"

appropriate to present in the

medium

3

fifth

of communication.

meaning of a composition

is

seemed

chapter material on music as a possible

And

clearly

as a

person's attitude toward the

forms an important part of his

of his over-all attitude toward that composition and

language chapter

it

its

taste,

composer, the

followed by two on musical taste— Chapter 6 on

the nature of taste and Chapter 7 on the several ways of measuring

The

discussion of the basic musical abilities and their

(Chaps.

and 9) might

8

for in a very real sense

suppose

abilities.

A

however.
is

all

perception, affection, and attitude pre-

There are other ways of viewing the problem,

They can be expressed only

his culture's taste,

inhibitory.

have appeared earlier in the book,

person's abilities will mature only

propitious.

own and

justifiably

it.

measurement

and

this taste

if

the social climate

in the context of a

man's

may prove stimulating or
may be thought to

In this limited sense, then, ability

13

THE PSYCHOLOGICAL APPROACH TO MUSIC
depend on

taste

and so deserve treatment,

The consideration of the appHcations
appears as the

book, after

what he

is

to medicine and industry

chapter, for before one tries to apply any

final

ledge he should

With

as it has in this

been considered.

taste has

make

certain that he has learned

all

know-

he can about

to apply.

this brief

introduction to the social psychology of music, let

us examine next the musical scale, the totality of those fixed (but
relatively placed) pitch positions

which the musician

uses in his

melodic and harmonic endeavors.

Notes
i]

as

C. K. Aldrich, "The Effect of a Synthetic Marihuana-Hke

Measured by the Seashore Test,"

M. Mezzrow and
2]

One

by G.

of the

B.

B.

Wolfe, Really

most extensive of the

Publ.

the Blues,

studies

Hlth

N.Y.,

Rep.,

Compound on

Random

59 (1944):
House, 1946.

After testing 33i'o Negroes of

431-433;

which compared Negroes and whites was

Johnson reported in "Musical Talent of the Negro," Mus. Superv.

81, 83, 96.

Musical Talent

Wash.,

fifth

J.,

grade, eighth grade, and college level on

the Seashore Measures of Musical Talent he states: "It becomes evident that the only

conclusion to be drawn from the data

is

that

!£ (1928):

that there are

no

significant differences

fair

between

whites and Negroes on those basic musical sensibilities measured by the Seashore tests."
3]

M. Ramm,

K.

"Personality Maladjustment

among Monotones,"

Smith Coll. Stud. Soc.

Wk., ij (1947): 264-284.
4]
£]

M.

F.

Meyer, The Psychology oj

By true beat or takt

of time signatures or
6]

M. W. Lund, "An

is

the Other-one,

Columbia, Mo., Missouri Book, 1922.

meant the pulsations which underlie phrase rhythms irrespective

number

of notes in the phrase.

Analysis of the 'True-beat' in Music," Doctoral Thesis, Stanford U.,

1939. In a somewhat similar analysis of the true beat of phonograph recordings, Hodgson

found that

slightly less than half of the

Although he had undertaken

measures

fell

between 60 and 70 per minute.

his researches in the belief that true beat

may be

causally

Hodgson now admits the impossibility of proving a causal relationWalter Hodgson, "Absolute Tempo," Mus. Teach. Nat. Assoc. Proc. 1949> 43 Ser.,

related to heart beat,
ship.

19^1: 158-169.
7]

W.

T. Bartholomew, "Baton Movements," Peahody Bull., 29, No.

Using a similar technique,

F.

the same composition there

Giese found that

when

34^

2 (1933): 37-39persons were asked to conduct for

were huge individual differences

in the style of conducting.

Giese 's data show some similarity in the pattern of conducting (a) the different works of

14

THE PSYCHOLOGICAL APPROACH TO MUSIC
the same conductor and (b) compositions of the same musical school ("Individuum

Epoch

Taktierbewegungen bei verschiedenen Komponisten," Arch.

in

Ges.

und

Psychol.,

go

(1934): 380-426).
8]

The most extensive work on the

State University of Iowa.
tains articles

vibrato was done at the Seashore laboratories of the

See the U. of

la. Stud. Psychol.

Mus., vol.

by E. Easeley, M. Hattwick, M. T. Hollinshead,

M. Metfessel, R. S.
Wagner; an earlier

(1932), which con-

N. Reger, D. A. Rothschild, H. G. Seashore, and A. H.

Miller, S.

research from this same laboratory was that of

Experimental Study of the Pitch Factor in Artistic Singing,"

230-259.

1

F. E. Linder, J. Tififin,

Excellent

work

M. Schoen, "An

Psychol. Monog., 31

(1922):

has also been done by L. Cheslock, "Introductory Study on

Violin Vibrato," Peabodj Cons. Mus. Res. Stud., No.

i

(193

i).

See also

W.

E.

Kock,

"On

the Principle of Uncertainty in Sound, "J. Acoust. Soc. Amer., 7 (1935'): 5^6-58; "Certain

Phenomena Accompanying a Frequency Vibrato, "J. Acoust. Soc. Amer., 8 (1936):
M. Small, "An Objective Analysis of Artistic Violin Performance," U. of la.
Psychol. Mus., 4 (1936): 172-231; J. R. Tolmie, "An Analysis of the Vibrato from

Subjective

23-25-; A.
Stud.

the Viewpoint of Frequency and Amplitude Modulation," J. Acoust. Soc. Amer., 7 (1935):
29—36; L. Sjostrom, "Experimentellphonetische Untersuchungen des Vibratophanomens

der Singstimme," Acta Oto-larjng. (Stockholm), Suppl., 47 (1948): 123-130.
9]

J. F.

Psjchol.,

10]

Corso and D. Lewis, "Preferred Rate and Extent of Frequency Vibrato, "y. Appl.

34 (1950): 206-212.

"Explanation" as used in this book

A phenomenon

"description."
variables to

which

it

is

popularly referred to as
11]

J.

P. Foley, Jr.,

J. Soc. Psychol., 12
I

2]

This "loss of

related.
its

is,

in a sense,

to be explained

Where

is

merely an extension of the concept

described in the context of the other

these latter are antecedent in time they are

"causes."

"The Occupational Conditioning of Preferential Auditory Tempo,"

(1940):

I

21-129.

know-how"

has been greatly exaggerated in the popular mind.

A. Saunders, "The Mechanical Action of Violins, "J. Acoust. Soc. Amer., 9 (1937):
13]
81-98. A. Small has shown that the "better" stringed instruments differ from the
F.

"poorer" largely in their emphasis on frequency bands below 2500 d.v. ("The Tone-color
[Timbre] of Stringed Instruments," Mus. Teach. Nat. Assoc, 1940 (1941): 35^4-360). See
also E.
14]

G. Richardson, "Orchestral Acoustics,"

Even the

"lost art" of

making old

1

5]

Data from personal communications
to

Month., 80, isss- 211-224.

Italian violin varnish is lost

Michelman, "Lost Art of Strad Varnish,"

author. The Oxford Companion

Sci.

Month., 81,

Sci.

as

i9i;'5:

no longer.

See

J.

221-223.

well as from one unpublished study by the

Music (P. A. Scholes, ed., London, Oxford U. Press,

1943, p. 988) reports that in London and in several other places there have been similar
failures to discriminate the tones of the older

16]

M. G.

from those of the newer instruments.

Rigg, "Favorable versus Unfavorable Propaganda in the Enjoyment of Music,"

J. Exp. Psychol,

38 (1948): 78-81.

"A Radio Test

17]

T. Geiger,

It is

conceivable that the long-continued use of the Geiger procedure might increase the

of Musical Taste," Puhl. Opin. Quart., 14 (1950): 45^3-460.

15

THE PSYCHOLOGICAL APPROACH TO MUSIC
number

of persons

titled "classical."
1

8]

habitually tune in

was well known

the

name

of

J.

S.

in the late

Bach that

degree of formal education
19]

on musical programs even when the music

The organist-composer Buxtehude, although

public,
it is

who

is

Unfortunately, the reported data do not cover this possibility.

is

largely

unknown

to the present-day lay

seventeenth and early eighteenth centuries.
familiar to

all

But

now

laymen who have had any appreciable

(p. 276).

H. M. Stanton, "Measurement of Musical Talent," U. of

la.

Stud. Psychol. Mus., 2

(1935): 1-14020]

Even

number

in the short period that scientists have

been observing musical behavior,

of changes in basic activity have been noted. Thus, the

of the great Schumann-Heink was quite acceptable in her day but causes

even amusement to those of us

who

listen,

on recordings,

a

manner of breath control

to

much

what appear

distress or

to

be her

periodic gasps.
21]

J.

Tiffin,

"The Role of Pitch and

Artists," U. of la. Stud. Psychol. Mus.,

22]

134.

16

P. E.

Vernon, "Method

1

Intensity in the Vocal Vibrato of Students

and

(1932): 134-165-.

in Musical Psychology," Amer. J. PsjchoL,

42 (1930): 127-

CHAPTER TWO

The

Social Psychology of

Musical Scales

iVlusic

is

made

of socially accepted patterns of sounds. These

sounds are either noisy, with no perceptible pitch, or they are tonal,

and can be located on

a

high— low continuum. The tones may differ in

loudness, timbre, duration, volume, density, and quite possibly in
still

other characteristics.^ While

all

pitches in the middle range are

of potential musical use, only a few appear in any one composition.
is,

convention limits the number and relative locations of the

pitches.

This chapter will consider the several attempts at pitch

That

limitation, matters studied in music

under the category of

scales.

The Tone Elements of Music
Unlike the music of some other cultures in which the sounds slide

up or down without discrete
alike, the
far

back

steps and the patterns are rarely twice

music of the West has been built around fixed tones, so

as

there are records.

These tones, the stepwise, ordered

arrangement of which constitutes

a scale,

have been tied to a variety

of frequencies with their exact pitch locations largely matters of local
tradition or of convenience to the musical performer.

example, of

little

theoretical importance

whether violin

A

It
is

is,

for

tuned

at

43 3-2 d.v. (Philharmonic pitch), at 435- d.v. (French pitch), or at
some other agreed-upon frequency.^ Of more importance to the
17

THE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF MUSICAL SCALES
musical structure, to the melody and harmony,

by an

interval's

two

tones.

The absolute

is

the ratio formed

of the spans

size

is

generally

not of paramount importance since a large span covering 4000-

8000

much

d.v. has

the same psychological effect as a small span

spreading from 100 to 200 d.v.

Both spans are termed octaves 3 and

are characterized by the ratio of

i

:

2

out of ratios that melody

It is

.

and harmony are constructed, and so long

as the ratios are

unchanged,

the notes can be raised or lowered without damage to the tune. Such

an alteration of the pitch locations merely

another register.

To understand

music, then, one must learn

the

shifts

melody

to

the basic facts of the psychology of

how

ratios

have been employed in scale

construction.

The tones of the

ratio

1:2, the octave, are easily produced on

pipes, they sound well together, and they are so easily confused

that identical letter

names have

been given them. 5 For

traditionally

these and perhaps other psychological and biophysical reasons, the

octave relationship was already recognized in the earliest documents
history has provided.

The

ratio of

i

:

2

(also

by the ancients. This
3

ratio

and those of

:4 (the musical fourth) were so well

2

:

written 1/2) holds for

were much played upon

lengths of pipe of constant bore, and pipes

(the musical fifth) and

3

known by

the time of Pytha-

goras (approximately ^^^o b.c.) that the diatonic (or seven white-

note) scale of his day was conceived of as being built with the octave

and the

fifth

(or the fourth)

on the

ratios

i

:

2

and

2

:

3

(or 3:4).

Scales of Ancient Times

PYTHAGOREAN
with

fifths (2: 3)

Next

G

down by

or

and counts up
C.

SCALE.

a scale position, say F,

a

is

musical

Pythagoras derived his scale by starting

and moving progressively up by musical

fourths (3:4).^ Thus,
fifth, i.e.,

if

one begins with F

by seven semitones, one comes to

reached, then D, A, E, and finally B.

Proceeding

THE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF MUSICAL SCALES
Since each

further, the several black notes are successively located.

of these scale points
in row^

to

is

next higher

its

64:96:144:216:324:486:729

(six

2:3, the ratios given

fifth as

of the table belovv^ obtain,

l

F:C:G:D:A:E:B

i.e.,

as

contiguous 2:3 ratios).

Scale of Pythagoras
C

C

D

E

F

96

216

486

2

384

3

256

432
288

486
324

64
512

1/1

8/9

64/81

3

27

Sharp

38

31

G

G

310

3/4
1

30(312)

35

B

C

144

324

729

192

2

576

768

384

648
432

729

3

486

512

4

2/3

16/27

128/243

1/2

5

9

81

729

6

32

Sharp

39

34

Note: For the purposes of this

They could,

vs^ritten in

A

F Sharp

341-3

243

33

A

Sharp

1

such.

D

D

1

Sharp

311

3
31

36

book the sharps of

been replaced by the

in fact, have

this table

flats

3'

have no significance

C

and D,

by adding together successive musical

fifths

here merely to indicate that there are chromatic scale steps between

and E, F and G,

G

and A, and

In building his scale
(see row^

1

A

and B.

and the paragraph above), Pythagoras covered almost four

octaves, that

is,

64

third octave, and
value, 729,

as

of D, E, G, A, and B. They are

is

:

i

5:1 2

taken

2 8 is
:

729

as a

one octave,
is

i

2 8

:

2 5^6

another,

a part of a fourth octave.

If

2

^6

:

5^ i

2

a

the largest

reference point and the values of the low^er

pitch locations are successively doubled until their magnitudes

fall

between 729 and the next lower octave, 364*5^, the letter steps are
now located within a single octave span (row 2). It is not customary,
however, to locate

C

at 384.

"Scientific pitch" places

and the other

letter locations are given in

rows 2 and 3

magnitude

differ in

row

3.

it at

2^6 cycles

While the

values of

they denote identical ratios. Thus,
19

THE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF MUSICAL SCALES
C:D

can be expressed either

reduced

ratio given in

row

4.

2^6:288, 384:432 or

as

Similarly,

C E

8:9, the

as

can be either 2^6:3 24,

:

384:486 or 64: 81.
should be noted that the removal of the powers of

It

2

series of ratios or frequencies disturbs the octave location but

letter arrangements. Thus, if a certain

other C's

vs^ill

the ratios of

row

row

the values in

A

3

25-6,

the dividing through

by

2

2's until

odd numbers

are reached reveals

2's.

clearly the Pythagorean philosophy of scale construction

"tone symbols" by

and

not the

given the value of

Now

row

yields a series of skeleton values in

I

is

a

be 128, 64, ^12, or other numbers found by dividing

or multiplying 2^6 by a succession of

more

C

from

Max Meyer. 7

6,

formed

which have been termed

5,

Pythagoras,

his scale

and

it

can

now be

seen from

by the use of the prime numbers

exclusively.^

SUGGESTION FROM ARISTOXENUS (ciRCA

35^0 B.C.).

the process of climbing by seven semitone steps, by

fifths,

is

If

con-

tinued from B, the accidentals are located (from B to F sharp, then
to

C

sharp,

point F

2: 3 ratio

sharp,

of the

the octave
fifths

G

D

i

:

2

fifth,

A

sharp) and finally the starting

3^^.

This value, derived from the

sharp and

reached, this time as

is

cannot coincide with any power of

requires.

2,

which

Obviously, then, the Pythagorean cycle of

cannot lead to a perfect octave.

Having noticed the imperfection of the Pythagorean system, the

Greek philosopher Aristoxenus suggested tempering the

make each

half-step equal in ratio to every other one.

later that such a

so

change would have made the

that twelve fifths

would have

pattern, i.e., into seven octaves.
this suggestion at the time,

20

S.

Bach's day (p. 2^).

We

shall see

small

enough

precisely into an octave

Although nothing practical came of

over the years theorists (Galileo, for one)

kept reviving the notion until
J.

fitted

fifth just

intervals to

it

was

finally

put into practical use in

THE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF MUSICAL SCALES
SUGGESTION FRO M PT O LE M Y (CI RC A 200 A.D. ),
Ptolemy
was one of several who felt that worthwhile scales might be conA

structed with prime
small

numbers other than

i

and

number might be employed, thought Ptolemy,

zation of the diatonic scale.

reached

as the result

It is

Any

3.

relatively

in the rationali-

clear that these conclusions

were not

of psychological research. Their chief import-

ance derives from the fact that other theoretically minded musicians
of a later date held similar views.
a

compromise

between

The

scale next to

Pythagorean

the

be considered

conception

and

is

the

Ptolemaic.

Scale ofJust Intonation

What

generally given in textbooks

is

psychology

as the diatonic scale

Pythagorean scale

a

of Western music

w^as

the scale of just

number

g.

modified by the introduction of ratios based on
This modification can be understood through

comparison of the tone symbols of the two

F, C,

is

In the construction of the scale of just intonation the

intonation.

the prime

on music, physics, and

G, and

D are identical. The

scales.

The symbols

for

tone symbol for the Pythagorean A,

81, was changed to

^, a multiple of

which, 80, approximates the 81

of the older scale.

As will be seen

later (p. 27), this

tively small change, but

it

affected the location of

that of E, the Pythagorean

a

is

B and

compara-

particularly

form of which had been creating some

difficulties for the musicians.

Over the
stress to
fitted

centuries.

Western music had been giving increasing

harmony. While the simpler

well this development, the 64:

E, gave particular trouble.

changed to the simpler

number

4.:

8

ratios of the
1

Pythagorean scale

ratio of the third,

So, in the course of time,
s-

its

from C to
ratio

was

This introduction of another prime

to rival the Pythagorean

3

caused the scientifically minded to

look to the overtone, or harmonic, series for help in justifying

this

21

THE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF MUSICAL SCALES
more

departure in scale construction. 9 For what could be

scientific,

thought they, than to construct out of the lower harmonics a scale
for

harmonic usage. In

became

fact,

the early scientists, and

enamored of the overtone notion of

so

that they attempted to preserve this just scale
to niake

it

the "scale of nature."

It

some

later ones,

scale rationalization

from

all

further change,

was held that early man had

consciously or unconsciously "recognized" the overtones and out of

them he had derived
It is

his musical scale. ^°

unfortunate for the theorizing of these overtone enthusiasts

that the "natural" or just-intoned scale has not

accepted.

On

been carved

the contrary, in a
in

quite

number of

"unnatural" ways

been universally

cultures the octave has

(p.

ig).

deriving the scale from overtones our ancestors

Then, too, in

would have had

to

recognize partial tones as high as the forty-eighth. Yet, in spite of the

evidence against the overtone theory

it

has served to raise the just

scale to the status of the diatonic scale of science.

Scale of Just Intonation
C

C

D

D

E

Sharp

F Sharp

F

2

24

27

3

256

288

30
320

341-3

4

1/1

8/9

4/S

3/4

S

3

27

15

1

6

31

X 3)

30

G

22

Sharp

33

G

Sharp

(5

A

A

32

Sharp

48

2

36

40

3

384

426-6

45
480

512

4

2/3

3/5

8/15

1/2

5
6

9

5

45

3

32

51

(15x3)

31

THE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF MUSICAL SCALES

A

Number 7

Scale with the Prime

A

composed of the prime numbers

scale

attempted by several people," but

group of musicians. ^^

large

A

i,

3,

and

g,

7 has

been

has never been adopted by any

it

possible scale of this type

is

described

in the table below. ^3

Scale with the Prime
C

C

D

D

7

E

F

2

16

18

20

21

3

256

288

320

336

4

1/1

8/9

4/5

16/21

1

9

5

3(

32

51

5
6

G

G

sharp

A

Sharp

A

2

24

27

3

384

432

4

2/3

16/27

5

3

6

31

Sharp

F Sharp

21

(7x3;

C

Sharp

B

(28)

30

32

480

512

8/15

1/2

27

15

(7)

33

1

3"

(5x3)

(71)

This scale differs from the others shown so far not only in
the prime 7 but also in the fact that

modulated up

which

a fifth (or

on F

fell

down

its

a fourth).

here

in the other scales

its

use of

tone symbols have been

That

falls

is,

the tone symbol

i

on C.

The Needjor Modulation

A

glance at the scale tables will

had whole steps with
steps) of 243:2^6.
8

:

9 and 9

number

7

:

:

o

made

smaller steps of
steps

15^:16.

that the Pythagorean scale

and smaller steps (not actual

The just-intoned

—and
had three whole
— 20:21 and
i

smaller steps
clear,

ratios of 8 9

show

i

^

scale
:

1

6

.

half-

had two whole steps

The

scale

with the prime

8:9, 9:10, and 7:8

—and two

All three of these scales,

direct modulation virtually impossible. Yet the

it

is

new
23

THE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF MUSICAL SCALES
harmonic music of the West demanded modulation, and
interval of 8

:

9 will not sound like

one of 9

were

in an era of modulation, any of these three scales

If,

kept, either the listener

would have

a do to re

10.

:

to

be

to adjust to serious mistunings

or the instrument builder would need so to alter his sound sources

many

that

auxiliary tones sharper or flatter than those already avail-

able could be played. ^4

common

and was

system of sharps,

burdened.

still

Unfortunately the latter practice becanie

cumbersome

at least in part responsible for the

flats,

and other signs with which Western music

is

As an aftermath has come the apparently unending

argument over the exact pitch location of the
sharps should be pitched higher than

flats,

signs, i.e.,

whether

or vice versa.

Mean- Tone Temperament ^^

We have

noted that music theorists from the time of Aristoxenus

on have advocated tempering the
theorizing

is

In

intervals.

with such

line

mean-tone temperament, which came into use

in

western Europe in the seventeenth century and was almost universally

accepted during the eighteenth until equal temperament

the day.

It is

a shaving or flatting of several of the fifths so that the

Pythagorean hope of a scale of

fifths

could actually be

mean-tone temperament, modulations within
keys were possible.

It

a

fulfilled.

With

few of the most used

was unfortunate, however, that what today

are similarly pitched sharps and

flats, e.g.,

C

sharp and

D

given different pitch locations and were regarded

still

won

flat,

were

as different

notes.

Scale of
C
3

24

Sharp

256

G
3

C

Mean-Tone Temperament

382-8

D

D

Sharp

286-1

G

Sharp

A
428-0

A

Sharp

E

F

320

342-4

B

C

478-6

512

F Sharp

THE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF MUSICAL SCALES
Equal Temperament
Equal

temperament

in

Western music was accomplished by

dividing the octave into twelve equal semitone steps, i.e., equal
so far as ratios

frequencies.^^

which

The

J.

S.

were concerned but not

This

scale

terms of the span of

in

temperament

of equal

the one

is

Bach fought and to which the modern piano

ratio of the

is

for

tuned.

semitone in the scale of equal temperament

is

i: 1-05^9.

According to musicological research,

tempered
at

occur in the Orient.

scales also

appears

it

It is

that

equally

said that the

Siamese

times divide their octave into seven equal steps and the Javanese

divide theirs into five equal steps.

Western Scale of Equal Temperament
C

C

Sharp

3

G

G

Sharp

D

Sharp

A

A

Sharp

430-3

383-5

3

D
287-3

256

UNITS OF RATIO MEASUREMENT.

E

F

322-5

341-6

B

C

483-2

512

F Sharp

Either the octave or the

semitone of the equally tempered scale could have served
ratio

measurement

if

a relatively large unit

as a unit

of

had been desired. Yasser

has urged the adoption of the decitone, the centitone and the milli-

tone (tenth, hundredth, and thousandth of the tone of the scale of
equal temperamentyas feasible units. ^7 As a small unit the thousandth
part of the octave has been suggested. In
is

the cent,

tempered

which

scale.

^^

is

a

most general

use,

however,

hundredth part of the semitone of the equally

The octave

in this system of

measurement equals

1200 cents, the semitone 200.
Let us from

now on

attempt to think in terms of these cent units

and examine the previously described

scales to see

how

perceptually

^5

THE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF MUSICAL SCALES
important the changes from one scale to the next really were.
first

we must

between

look

at thresholds,

man's

at

ability to discriminate

intervals of differing size.

The Stability of Intervals
Intervals may be distorted, made somewhat
without

But

loss of identity.

It is

larger or smaller,

indeed fortunate that

this

is

true, for

otherwise the transition from the just-intoned scale to the mean-tone
or the equally tempered mi^ht have been
plish.

how much

Just

more

interval distortion can

difficult to

difficult to say, since the researches in this area are

But

agreement.
of the

fifth is

it

human

the

fifth

scales.

it

is

not entirely in

does appear that for most people the uniqueness

disturbed

more

easily than that of the third or sixth.

Happily enough, the historical changes were in line with
of

accom-

be tolerated

this aspect

psychophysiology. As will be noted in the following table,

was kept

at

approximately 700 cents in

Even the Siamese and Javanese had
approximating the

be thought of

as

approximately

5-00 cents.

On

the Western

all

which might
The fourth was kept at

scale points

fifth.

the other hand, the third, sixth, and

more extensively from scale to scale.
work by Pratt and his associates shows that

seventh varied
Careful

for perceiving changes in interval quality
in the

is

approximately 20 cents

middle of the tonal range. ^9 This value checks with the findings

of the present author,

between the

fifths

whose student

the seconds.

The

difference

mean-tone seventh, which
perceived.

accuracy a

subjects hear

no difference

of the several scales, between the several fourths

(except perhaps where the "7" scale

is

concerned), or even between

between the Pythagorean seventh and the
is

27 cents

(iiio— 1083),

is

occasionally

But more subjects discriminate with better than chance

number

of the other larger differences.

discussed are, then, perceptually as well as

26

the threshold

The

scales so far

mathematically different

THE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF MUSICAL SCALES
some contemporary laymen. However,

to

would tend

to

make most

the vibrato,

when present,

of these differences imperceptible.

Values in Cents of Scale Intervals
D

E

F

G

A

B

C

204
204

408

498

702

906

1110

1200

Just

386

498 f

702

1088

1200

M.T.T.

193

386

503

697

884
890

1083

1200

E.T.

200

400

500

700

900

1100

1200

C
pjth.

514 686

343

171

Siam.

480

240

Javan.*
"7"

204

*

has been found that the

It

between the

intervals

Tuning
•j-

in a

386

Ganda Harp,"

T. E. Simonton, in

"A

Ganda

Nature,

720
702

471

strings all equal

857

960

P.

1200

906

eight-string harp of Africa

240 cents (K.

1200

1029
1088
is

so

1200

tuned that the

Wachsmann, "An Equal-Stepped

l6s (London, 1950): 40-41).
Ratio Chromatic Scale, "J.

New Integral

Acoust. Soc. Amer.,

2S

{^9Sl)' 1167-1170, has devised a chromatic scale with a perfectly symmetrical arrangement of just fourths. He claims that it combines the advantages of the Pythagorean, the
just-intoned, and the equally

scales.

possible that the musicians of several centuries past had even

It is

more

tempered

difficulty than

we

in perceiving the differences, since their

tone-producing sources were markedly cruder than the apparatus of
today.

But of course the theoreticians expected to find differences.

Believing as they did in the perfection of the just-intoned scale, they

undoubtedly imagined greater differences than they actually perceived.

PITCH DISCRIMINATION.
compare

Let US supposc that one

is

asked to

single tones rather than intervals, for example, the Pytha-

gorean and mean-tone D's. Then, of course, pitch discrimination

comes

into play, and this sensitivity

is

detect differences between intervals.

can be discriminated by
So,

many

far

keener than that needed to

But even though the two D's

people, the two major seconds cannot.

despite the fact that pitch discrimination and tolerance for

interval change are related, they are not the same.

27

THE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF MUSICAL SCALES
Equal Temperament, Our Frame of Reference
Western music

is

today tied rather closely to the piano, and pianos

are tuned to equal temperament. Yet there

still

are theorists

who

maintain that keen-eared musicians "sense" equal temperament to be
a

mere compromise. These

just intonation

The author

whenever

sensitive people will, they hold,

possible to do so.^°

it is

many

has not had the privilege of testing

sensitive people.

employ

His subjects have,

in the main,

musically

been laymen,

amateur musicians, and teachers of music, and to them he has presented a variety of scales.
scales, the
it is

He

has found that

commonest response when the

if

he does not label the

just scale

played

is

odd, interesting, and not particularly displeasing.

anyone prefer

it

to the equally tempered.

But

if

is

that

Rarely does

the just scale

is

presented and labeled, a few of the better-trained musicians, feeling
perhaps that they should prefer

harmonic

it,

may

declare that they like

possibilities, that its thirds are so

much

better, etc.

its

When

the scales have not been identified, the typical listener appears to feel

more comfortable with

the scale of equal temperament, the scale of

his present culture period.

He

does not "unconsciously" think in

terms of just intonation.
It is

true that the great Helmholtz quotes cases

where

violinists

reverted to just intonation whenever their playing was unaccompanied.^^ These observations

modern

may or may not have been

research paints a different picture.

unaccompanied playing of six professional
been anticipated, the accuracy of the

valid.

But

Greene studied the

violinists. ^^

As might have

intervals varied considerably.

But the mean value of each interval was closer to what was advocated

by Pythagoras than to that of just intonation.

Later

work by

Nickerson on unaccompanied solo and ensemble performances of the

same melody gave very similar

28

results. ^3

THE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF MUSICAL SCALES
Other Possible Scales
So

far

notes.

we

have discussed only the Western diatonic scale of seven

While

the basic scale of the West, several other scales

it is

One

have had a substantial degree of popularity.

D

pentatonic, w^here only the F, G, A, C, and

C

sharp, and

D

sharp, i.e., spans of

steps.

this scale

and many Chinese

Even

five

at a

it is

when

time

today,

G

semitones,

2,

it

the diatonic scale was

was allowable

to

make

diatonic tones there was
i.e.,

more

When

The

adhered to

they were called,

added to the seven
scale of the

and poly tonal

employed by Debussy and others

D, E, F sharp,

e.g., C,

is

G

sharp,

A

many

sharp, C.

people,

in
It

who

obviously not their accustomed major scale

—which of course

Scriabin has used a scale
flat

finally 2

facilitated their acceptance.

believe that since this

B

as

rise of atonal

has been found that this scale seems strange to

must be minor

rigidly

formed the twelve-semitone

accidentals have also been

whole-tone music,

it

sharp,

the dodecuple. These twelve tones have been

freely used as a scale.

music has greatly

The

and

3

A

occasional use of any one of

to give variety or color.

modern piano,
more and more

sharp,

if

as well.

other notes not in the scale. These accidentals,

were thought

the

is

A number of well-known Scottish tunes are based on

semitone

than

2

—F

sharp,

one prefers, the black notes of the piano

of these'

are employed, or,

whose

it is

not

(p. 89).

steps are C,

D, E, F sharp, A,

to give a distinctive character to his music. Perhaps others will

follow him in devising
Scales

made up

still

other scale steps

as their

personal idiom.

of a variety of microtones have been suggested at

one time or another. One of the most bizarre was that of the
century Chinese, Chien Lohtze,

who proposed

match the 360 days he thought made up the
that this theorist had

a

fifth-

360-note scale to

year.

It is

quite clear

done no research on the psychology of human

perception.

29

THE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF MUSICAL SCALES
The Hindus have a 2 2 -note scale which can be sung with considerable accuracy. The Moravian musician Haba, who has advocated
splitting the semitone into a number of microtones, claims to be able
to sing intervals as small as one-sixtieth of an octave.
is

able to do this, he

Our

interest

would

But even

clearly not in freakish virtuosity but in those

is

extensions to our present scale that could prove successful.

on

development, partly because our music system

semitone base and the quarter-tone

a

A number

he

lack appreciative listeners.

of musicians and psychologists have seen the quarter-tone
logical

if

is

A number
as

the next

now

operates

half of the semitone.

of quarter-tone pianos and organs have been built and a

considerable body of quarter-tone music has been written. ^4
It
is

will be recalled that the threshold for interval discrimination

approximately 20 cents.

But

this

is

the value at which Pratt's

observers could detect the differences only half of the time.

To be

completely functional, however, an interval should be discriminated
properly not half of the time but

the time.

all

The value

for the

smallest interval that will be perceived as different 100 per cent of

the time Pratt has found to be about ^o cents, the span of the

quarter-tone in the equally tempered scale. ^5
are psychologically feasible.

But no interval

Quarter- tones, then,

much

smaller than the

quarter-tone would seem to be acceptable except to the person of
extraordinary sensitivity.^^

An

interesting demonstration for the musically unsophisticated

to play

one octave

A common
played.

answer

and to ask what has been heard.

that the chromatic scale of

Most persons

intervals pleasing.
is

in quarter-tones
is

will find at least a

two octaves

few of the quarter-tone

C

to a tone

between the minor

and major third, an interval which approximates one
fact, this interval is typically

of the seconds or

30

has been

Generally, the most pleasing quarter-tone span

the one that extends from a bass of

music. In

is

sevenths. ^7

in

Siamese

rated as pleasanter than either

THE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF MUSICAL SCALES

Summary
In this chapter

we

have seen that the basic scale of Western music

has been for centuries the seven-note diatonic, w^hose precise ratios

have varied from time to time. The Pythagoreans had held that a
succession oftwelve musical fifths of ratio

seven octaves,

i.e.,

there

would be

2

:

3

could be arranged to cover

a return to the starting letter

seven octaves higher. Hence the primes

and

i

3

,

some

which alone appear

in the ratio of their musical fifth, were, according to the Pythagoreans,

the only primes needed in the building of the diatonic scale.
It

was early noted that the twelve Pythagorean

fifths

were

a trifle

too large to squeeze into seven octaves, and again and again there arose

The

the hope that other primes might be used in the scale ratios.
first

to be accepted

just intonation.

was the

g,

The addition of

flatting the third, the sixth,

ratios facilitated

which appeared

work

this

in

what was termed

prime was accomplished by

and the seventh.

Since the

harmony, there developed

in

new

scale

a school

of

thought which subscribed to the notion that the just-intoned scale
steps

had been borrowed from positions on the overtone or harmonic
held that

It w^as

series.

man had

sciously, the tonal partials
scale.

Such

a

recognized, consciously or uncon-

and had made use of them in creating

his

on the Deity

for

philosophy inspired theologians to

call

support of His scale and led early scientists to regard the just scale
"natural."

The

scale

became

so

honored that

all efl^orts

to alter

it

met

with tremendous opposition, even though the arguments for

"God-given quality" and

its

"naturalness"

Although there were attempts to bring
£,

the

more

lasting changes to the scale

equal temperament.

The former made

were
in

as

its

invalid.

primes other than

3

and

came with mean-tone and

a limited

modulation possible,

while equal temperament allowed for free modulation ranging

through
octave

is

all

keys.

In equal

temperament,

as the

term implies, the

divided into twelve equal intervals.
31

THE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF MUSICAL SCALES
Taking the cent, or hundredth part of the semitone of the equally

tempered

measurement,

scale, as the unit of scale

tolerance for interval change

is

of the scale changes had been less than

probably caused but

was found that

it

approximately 20 cents. Since many
2

o cents in

magnitude they

except to a few theorists and

little difficulty

musically sensitive persons. Scale changes above threshold magnitude

do

exist,

however, and most persons can

tell that

the several scales

are not identical.

There

is

no evidence

that

we

hesitantly accept equal

for practical reasons only, i.e., solely because

and

in

temperament

allows modulation,

terms of just intona-

Psychological research discloses no outstanding preference for

tion.

the

do our musical thinking

really prefer to

it

"natural"

violinists

and suggests that whenever unaccompanied

scale

deviate in their playing from equal

temperament

their

intervals are actually closer to those of Pythagorean intonation.

the other hand, there

Mursell

menon

says,

of

"Any

no reason to suppose that the diatonic

is

of equal temperament

Western music's

is

scale

is

final scale

a construct of the social

social agreement. "^^

The

diatonic has had

pentatonic, dodecuple, whole-tone, etc.

—and

reasonable scale units.

pheno-

a

rivals

circles

is

Research indicates that microtones

smaller than ^o cents are

more

now

and therefore are perceptually

intervals

as

some

as

Quarter-tone spans of

being modified to include quarter-tones.
^o cents can be heard

in

many

scale

For

form.

mind,

On

much

rarely perceived as qualitatively

unique by the typical listener and hence are not functional for
music.

A number

of scale properties

still

they can better be understood after
to the

interval,

in

particular

melody. The interval and
chapter.

3^

its

to

remain to be described.

more

the

But

attention has been given

role

role, then, will

the

interval plays in

concern us in the next

THE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF MUSICAL SCALES
Notes
For an authoritative consideration of tonal characteristics see

i]

Handbook of Experimental Psychology, N.Y., Wiley,

The

2]

Stevens,

S.

S.

ed.,

igi'i.

value 435- d.v. (double vibrations), or 435- cycles, indicates that the sound-giving

body, be

it

a string, a fork, or a

column of

air, is

vibrating 43 £ times a second.

For the sake of convenience, the names given the intervals in Western music will be

3]

employed here even though the terms have proved to be definitely misleading to the
The octave, so called because it makes use of eight w^hite notes on the

unsophisticated.

piano,

better described as a span of 12 semitones; the

is

named

notes, as seven semitones; the fourth,

fifth,

which involves

five

white

for the four white notes used, as five

semitones; etc.

who was

C. Stumpf, a psychologist

4]

perhaps the

to

first

show

any other two tones.

See his Tonpsjchologie, Leipzig, Hirzel, 1883, 1890.

frequency ratio of which

The fourth

6]

is

was

two tones of an octave more than

600 Pope Gregory the Great ordered a single name to be given

In A.D.

g]

a pioneer in the area of musical aesthetics,

that naive subjects confuse the

is

i

:

2

some power of

(or to

merely the octave minus the

i.e.,

fifth,

all

tones the

2).

12 semitones

minus

7 equals

£ semitones.
7]

M.

8]

Since F

^0

Qj-

9]

A

F.

Meyer, The Musician
is

Arithmetic, Boston, Ditson, 1929.
fifth

above

A

sharp,

vibrating body, be

it

a string or a

own

pitch(octave plus a

column of air,

body gives forth

fifth), etc.

its

fundamental to these harmonics are
10]

can be either

vibrates not only as a

A somewhat

whole but

also

unique tone (the octave), one-third gives

These auxiliary tones are termed overtones and with

The

the fundamental tone constitute the series of partials or harmonics.

it

it

^12

in sections. Thus, one-half the
its

s

not only the starting point but also the

as

i

:

2,

1:3, 1:4,

i

similar concept occurred in the naive

:

ratios of the

^, etc.

geometry of architecture. Here,

was held, man was somehow psychobiologically in tune with the root squares, the

golden sections, and the other "God-blessed" forms used so

much by

the Greeks of the

classical period.
1

1]

and
I

2]

The experimental

aesthetician

M.

F.

7 in a neurological interpretation of
It is

has

made

use of the small primes 1,3,

Western music of prime numbers not

offered was that since

below, before,

God

after, right,

in His

Wisdom had

and

—the number

left

in the series

i

to

5^.

The reason

arranged for only six directions
6

had metaphysical

prime would be to go contrary to God's Will. The

classical

rationalization although they spoke of only five "proper" directions
after,

f,

musical intervals (The Musician's Arithmetic).

of some sociopsychological interest that in the sixteenth century a taboo existed

against the use in

a larger

Meyer

—above,

significance.

Chinese had

— up,

To

use

a similar

down, before,

and center.

33

THE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF MUSICAL SCALES
A

13]

note
14]

1

scale

W.

developed by H.

Poole, an American organ builder (see

Meyer

reference,

1).

That the listener will adjust to mistunings can be demonstrated.

pologists have reported that their pitch discrimination

is

contact with alien intervals, and one such claim has been checked.

whose musical instruments tend

the inland Chinese,

Several anthro-

temporarily impaired after long
It

has been noted that

make poorer

to be carelessly tuned,

scores on pitch discrimination tests than do the coastal and American-born Chinese, for

whom

exact tuning

is

more

a part of the culture (P.

R. Farnsworth,

"An

and Experimental Study of the Seashore-Kwalwasser Test Battery,"

Critical,

Historical,

Genet. Psychol.

Monogr., 9 (1931): 29I-393-)
i^]
die

H. H. Drager, "Zur mitteltonigen und gleichschwebenden Temperatur,"

Juli 19 SO, pp.

16]

Bachtagung der

Wissenschaftliche

O.

Gesellschaft

Bericht iiber

23.

bis

Jacobsen defends the idea that the tempered and the "natural" scale each has

I.

Tempered

("Harmonic Blending

26.

Yasser, A Theory of Evolving Tonality, N.Y.,

J.

1

Another unit of

ratio

measurement

its

in the Natural versus the

126—132.)

Scale," J. Musical., 2 (1941):

17]

4

Leipzig,

389-404.

place for contemporary performance.

8]

fur Musikforschung,

is

Amer.

Lib. Musicol., 1932.

the savart, which has a span of approximately

cents.

19]

C. C. Pratt, "Quarter-Tone Music,"

Thresholds for intervals

from musical,

i.e.,

Hence,

observers.

y. genet. Psychol.,

made from pure

35 (1928): 286-293.

tones are slightly different from those formed

impure, tones. Pratt's data were elicited from psychologically trained
it is

more typical listener would have a
The reader should not confuse threshold and

reasonable to suppose that the

threshold somewhat above

20 cents.

terms which are negatively related. Where the listener's threshold is low,
where he perceives the interval quality as changed after very little expansion or

sensitivity,
i.e.,

contraction of the pitch span, he
20]

Other

theorists

tone intonation in mind but
21]

H.

L. F.

is

said to

be highly

sensitive.

worry because much highly rated music was composed with mean-

Helmholtz, On

now

is

played with equal temperament.

the Sensations of Tone,

N.Y., Longmans, Green, 1912,

p.

486.

See also A. C. Roncalio, "Just and Equal Temperament," J. Musical., 3 (1941): 120—122.
22]
23]

P. C. Greene, "Violin Intonation," J. Acoust. Sac. Amer., 9 (1937),
J.

F.

Melody,"
24]

J. Acoust. Sac. Amer., 21

Max Meyer

{The Musician

s

(1949): 593-^9^.

Arithmetic)

and others have long advocated the introduction

of quarter-tones into our music system and have argued that a
signatures and the usual signs
staff lines

would be

arranged

34

as are

is

in order.

See also P.

2S3 (19^2): 12^-143.

C. C. Pratt, op.

cit.

One

such

new

staff has

staff

which omits key

been suggested with the

The only sign needed in this new staff
Moon, "A Proposed Musical Notation,"

the piano's black keys.

for the quarter-tone.

J. Franklin Inst.,

2^]

43-44.

Nickerson, "Intonation of Solo and Ensemble Performance of the Same

THE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF MUSICAL SCALES
26]

An

organ with a 31 -note scale to carry out the harmonic ideas of the seventeenth-

century mathematician Christiaan Huygens has been built by the Dutch physicist, A. D.

Fokker ("Equal Temperament and the Thirty-one-keyed Organ,"

Sci.

Month., 81 {i3SS)'-

161-166). The basic interval of approximately 39 cents would appear to be on the very
border of practicality at best.
27]

P. R.

Farnsworth and C.

J. Appl. Psychol., 12 (1928):

28]

J. L.

F. Voegelin,

"Dyad Preferences

at Different Intensities,"

148-1^1.

Mursell, "Psychology and the Problem of the Scale," Music. Quart., 32 (1946):

^64-^73-

3?
3-2

CHAPTER THREE

The

In

Interval

the preceding chapter

music are the tonal

it

was shown that the building blocks of

intervals. This chapter will

continue the exami-

nation of these musical constituents and will pay special attention to
a

number

of characteristics they have been said to possess/

Distinctive Quale
It

goes almost without saying that each interval has

distinctive quale,

recognized.

We

the psychological characteristics by

have already mentioned the

own

its

which

fact that,

it

is

physically

may be a number of things. The tones of the
example, may have frequencies of 2^6 and 432, 25^6

speaking, the interval

major

sixth, for

384 and 648, or even ^i

and 426-6, 2^6 and 430*

3,

mention only

a

But

configurations

have a psychological quale in

sixthness.

within

a

few.

The major

all

sixth,

like all

margin of span tolerance

A number

of researchers

who

2

and 864 d.v.

,

to

these physically different stimulus

common,

a

major

other intervals, has stability

(p. 26).

are interested in phenomenological

description have attempted to describe verbally the several interval
characteristics.

The

results have

not been strikingly successful.

It is

unlikely that an octave rendered by a flute will be described in the

same terms
36

as will

the identical interval presented by a piano, a

THEINTERVAL
violin, or a tuba.

However, the reader can check
on

to intervals played

a variety of instruments

his

ov^n reactions

and see

if

the

fifth

always sounds dilute, hollow, and harsh; the fourth rich, harsh, and
coarse; the second gritty and grating; the third

mellow and sweet;

the sixth luscious and juicy-mellow;

the seventh astringent and

sharp-rough; and the octave smooth,

as

the

Edmunds and Smith

observers found them.^

The reader

will probably already

know

can improve his ability to recognize the
also

know

that descending intervals are

than the ascending (p. 39).
find that he, like

If

Ortmann's

he

tests

subjects, 3

much practice he
intervals. He will possibly
more difficult to recognize
that by

himself he will very likely
is

relatively

poorer in

his

recognition of minor intervals and particularly of minor sixths,
sevenths, and thirds, and, like Maltzew's observers, 4 poor also in the
identification of tritones.
in the

middle

Recognition, he will find, tends to be best

register, the tonal range

most used

in music.

But

whether the greater use of the middle range has made recognition
better or better recognition has forced greater usage

is

as

yet

unanswered.

Vibrato, Tremolo,

and

Trill

Vibrato concerns physical spans too small to be perceived

Although the

intervals.

definite pitch spans

reacts as

if

violinist or vocalist

whenever he employs

produces

as

such but rather

them, and he continues for

a

as

as

of

his vibrato, the listener

he were hearing embellished single tones.

hear the two pitches

a series

He

does not

one pitch midway between

time to hear a single tone even though

the performer oscillates his tone over a wider and wider pitch
span.

Whenever

the embellishment has

extreme, the effect

is

more

become

likely to

so

marked

be termed

a

as to

be quite

tremolo than

a

37

THE INTERVAL
vibrato, although

the two.

If

no

definite

the extent of oscillation

between three-quarters of
ceases to hear a single
tion

boundary Hne can be drawn to separate

a

between two tones,

emerges

as qualitatively different

pitches are

at vibrato rate.

He

reaches his interval.

heard

In

somewhere

instead perceives an oscilla-

a successive interval,

now

still,

tone and two whole tones, the listener

wobbly tone and

The two
even

becomes greater

or

The trill,

trill.

then,

from the tremolo and the vibrato.

as separate tones

making

a trill the

which may

alternate

performer often over-

does this to compensate for the fact that his

listener tends to underestimate the size of the trill just as

he does

the extent of the vibrato.

Apparent Pitch of Intervals
After stating that an interval has

seem paradoxical to speak of

relative pitch

a

which even

pitches,

it

is

an entity and

nai've listeners

as

may

were

interval pitch as if the interval

Yet each interval

single tone.

two boundary

a

such possesses a

can recognize.

Many years ago while at work on the pitch of intervals, Stumpf,
German psychologist, proposed a principle which he supposed

had universal application.^

component of an
upper, that
that

it

is,

interval

He
is

held that the pitch of the lower

always dominant over that

of the

much more

obvious

that the pitch of the

lower

is

so

has the greater effect on the pitch of the interval con-

figuration.

In his experiments

to his listeners, one

he presented two octave intervals

whose frequency span was approximately from

130 to 260 d.v. and the other from 260 to

reported that

it

was the apparent pitch of the

was more unlike the pitch of 260

d.v.

They seemed, then,

more

to be attending

5^20.
first

His audience
interval

which

(approximately middle C).
to the pitches of the

lower

components.
Stumpf's generalization seems in line with the dictum of the

THE INTERVA L
harmonists
is

who

state that the

normal orientation of

a tonal pattern

from below upward. To quote Watt: "Rising makes the impression

We

of tonal recession, falling, that of approach.
voluntarily

Perhaps

begin a scale

from below, not from above, and we end

this

tendency toward upward movement

it

is

below

in-

again. "7

responsible for

the fact, mentioned above, that ascending intervals are easier to

recognize than the descending.

Which

is

more

unlike middle

C

Which

in pitch?

higher in pitch?

is

IC

-er^

or

w
Unfortunately, the British psychologist Valentine was unable to
verify this principle of

Stumpf 's,

of the higher interval

as

more

for his subjects regarded the pitch

unlike that of 260 d.v.^

When

he

further asked for a comparison of the pitch of a certain octave with
that of a smaller interval

which

lay

wholly within

this octave, his

subjects voted for the octave as the higher in pitch. Thus,

apparent that his subjects were, in each instance,

it is

quite

more impressed by

the higher pitch.

Puzzled by these conflicting sets of data,

performed two experiments along similar
1920's and the other fifteen years later. 9

the present writer

lines,

one

In his

in the early

researches he

attempted to learn whether these divergent findings might not some-

how be
in his

related to the fact that

Stumpf had used musically naive men

experiments while the majority of Valentine's subjects had

been schoolchildren with some musical

training.

It

was the author's

theory that children, sopranos, and well-trained musicians generally
39

THE INTERVAL
have had so

upper
tones

much

experience in melody hunting, in looking

lines of their

now

To

stand out for them.

verify this idea that

melody hunting

unmusical persons, sopranos, contraltos and
resulting data

showed

an important variable

is

the writer tested relatively

in establishing the pitch of intervals,

The

the

at

music, that the higher pitches of a complex of

and male singers.

altos,

that the relatively unmusical,

and to some

The

extent the male singers, voted in line with Stumpfian theory.
sopranos, on the other hand, and

many

of the other singers of both

sexes behaved as had Valentine's schoolchildren.

Clearly,

then,

Stumpf's rule must be modified to embrace the effects of training
in

melody hunting.

Stumpf was correct

of the lower component of an interval
musically untrained and for

higher component which

some

is

basses.

in stating that the pitch

dominant, but only for the

For other persons

more markedly

it is

the

affects the pitch of the

interval.

The Major-Minor

Effect

Misled by the terms "major" and "minor," many people assume
that the

major

sixth, for

and the minor sixth

a

example,

calls forth a

minor or gloomy-feeling tone.

demonstrated, however, that intervals,

minor

major or joyous

characteristics.

as such,

a

Moreover, the major and minor chords, the

major and

better, therefore, had

the small
labels

number

a

more

minor

interval.

It

(p. 88), are

each

would have been

far

writers followed the lead of Alchin and

of bolder harmonists

who

have changed the interval

from "major" and "minor" to "large" and "small. "^° To term

a sixth a "large sixth"

characteristics.

40

can easily be

have no major or

musical structures which do possess such attributes

composed of

It

affect

is

to

impute to

it

no

special psychological

THE INTERVAL
Finality Effects

A

simple demonstration of

finality effect

made by running

can be

over a succession of C's and F's on the keyboard and asking the
listeners for an appropriate stopping point.
in Western music there will be

almost invariably be

one of the C's

made on an

with

some have

C

as the first

seem more

that ordinarily

The

argument.

stop will

Yet the vote would have been for

C

this note,

restful,
is

The repeated

scale.

note will make

C

playing of this

the tonic or keynote, which

or intervals and chords with

more

finished,

not regarded

characteristic of finality

We

listeners steeped

"magnet tone." Because of the emphasis on C,

called a

melodies ending on
will

Among

the demonstration had been immediately preceded

if

by several renditions of the
scale

F.

little

if it is

more

as at all

final.

it as

all

a bass,

Even an ending

restful will take

listened to as an ending

see here, then, a principle of return, a desire to

on

this

many times. ^^
come back to

an emphasized note.

may well ask, why was so much emphasis on C
needed to make it the more appropriate ending? Why was a stop on
F otherwise the iTiore final? The answer would appear to depend on
the way Western scales are constructed. In the key of F, C is a
perfect fifth above F; and the fifth, or dominant, is without much
But, the reader

doubt the second most important note of the

C

scale.

boundary note of both the major and minor chords

A

flat,

C).

On

the other hand, F

fourth above C) in the key of
often than

C

is

heard

as

C

is

and

is

the upper

(F,

A, C; F,

only the subdominant (a perfect
is

heard in that capacity

the dominant in the key of F.

experience leads us to structure C's and F's with F

as

far less

Hence,

the tonic, with

the notes in the key of F.
If

the listener had heard a succession of unemphasized C's and

G's, his inclination

would have been

appropriate ending. This time

it is

to vote for a

C which

is

C

as the

more

the subdominant in the

41

THE INTE R VAL
key of G and

G the dominant in the

key of C. Therefore

C

is

the

more

hkely keynote and gives the better ending.

A

second principle which operates in inducing finahty

that of faUing inflection. ^^

Just as in

with an unresolved question and

music there

falls

many

with an assertion, so in Western

lower tone of

This

phenomenon

has appeared in the data of a host of re-

quite possibly linked to the fact, mentioned earlier,

searchers and

is

that a scale

normally begun from below and

The
It

is

effect of falling inflection

enhanced by

is

also appears that size of interval

finality.

intervals

is

a

ended

is

size

of successive

steadily decreases the

But with chords the effect of

may be

descent.

related to restfulness and

from one to eleven semitones rather

different, for the larger chords

as a

slowing of the tempo.

Thus, Zener has shown that increasing the

finality effect. ^3

a succes-

descending melodic

sively played interval or the lowest point of a
line.

is

languages the voice rises

a feeling of resolution in the

is

effects

as restful as

span

is

quite

or even

more

size of

restful than the smaller.

Striking finality effects can be obtained by playing a successive
interval

(or broken

chord or melody) where the ratio of the

frequency of one of the tones
multiple of

2 is

of the perfect
rest.

to

is

to that of the other as

some other number. For example,

fifth,

whose

Thispower-of-2 effect

ratio
is

is

2:3, the

2

is

2

or some

in the interval

the point of

more

often termed the Lipps-Meyer law after

the eminent psychologists Lipps^^ and Meyer, ^5

who

first

worked

in

the area and postulated similar theories to explain the phenomenon.

Much

of the factual side of the Lipps-Meyer law

is

unquestioned,

but considerable controversy has arisen over the explanation of what
it

describes.

w^hich makes

Is
it

there

some neurological reason

universally applicable, or should

for the
it

phenomenon

be subsumed under

the co-principles of emphasis and return and applied only to certain
cultures? Thirty years or
to

42

more

ago, the present author

was inclined

accept a neurological position similar to that of the earlier

THE INTERVAL
In fact, in researches

theorists.

he had found

lesser but

the ratio symbol

with

still

undertaken in the 1920's, he thought

measurable

smaller effects with

3,

In these experiments

7.^^

finality effects associated

and

£,

still

with

smaller effects

he had employed stimuli tuned

in

equal temperament which he hoped would approximate the symbols
3,

g

and

7.

A

decade later he was able to use tones pitched to just

intonation, i.e., with quite exact tuning. ^7 But this time these lesser
finality effects did

not appear. ^^ His inability to validate the earlier

work strengthened

his belief that the neurological factor, as reflected

in these primes,

Among
effect

is

those

was not of major importance

who

in finality effects.

have attempted to show that the Lipps-Meyer

the resultant of cultural conditioning rather than biophysical

structure

is

Updegraff, whose research was arranged to determine

whether the conventional resolution of

"power

of 2"

former,

she

was the more
found,

a

influential in

suggested

harmony or the

determining

The
More

finalty.^9

appeared to be somewhat stronger.

important, perhaps, was her discovery that Chinese students regarded
*

Chinese melodies

own

for their

as

more

melodies.

finished

and

restful

while Americans voted

In other words, the decisive element for

Updegraff 's subjects was familiarity, not the pitch relationships of the
endings.

Others have found that American-reared Chinese give responses

much

like those of Caucasian

Americans.

Orientals recently from

the coast of China feel the finality effects of our Western music less

keenly than American

where our

interior,

Chinese,

style of

music

while those from the
is

Chinese

not so well known, do not share

out notions of finality. Because of these racial-national differences in
response,
likely

it

would appear

that cultural conditioning

explanation for the Lipps-Meyer power-of-2

is

the

effect.

more
It

is

conceivable, however, that in addition to the "pulling power" which
practice

would

give the tone symbol 2, there

may

also

important neurological or other biophysical causation.

be some

less

^"^

43

THE INTERVAL
Interval Resolutions

Harmony textbooks teach that the successively played intervals
that make up the diatonic scale can be divided into those that are
"restless" or unresolved and others that have repose and finality. The
table

below shows the

Any other

system.

traditional resolutions in the tonic sol-fa

intervals,

such

as

do—re or do-ti, yield a "restless"

or unresolved effect.

Resolutions
Tonic Sol- Fa System
re to

do

re to

mi

Scale Letter in Key of

Ja to mi

G
A

to

C

la to sol

to

G

to do

B

to

C

sol

ti

C

D to C
D to E
FtoE

to do

Half of the resolutions listed in the table are quite obviously

C has been

examples of the principle of return. Wherever
emphasized to achieve keynote

on C,

that

is,

on

do,

status, all successive intervals

will display the

finality

descending major second re— do, the descending

a

symbol

interval

or ascending
return to the

If

scaled in just intonation the intervals are seen to end

2

tone (9 to

(27

to

3,

2

,

3

or 9 to

to

2

2).^^

,

and

15-

to

(It

as

on

does the la-sol

It is

interesting to note
it

were, the

will be recalled that the ratios of equally

tempered

that performers often sharp the

resolution.

2

)

These resolutions, then, can be

described in terms of the Lipps-Meyer effect.

scales

all

ending

Thus, the

effect.
fifth

fourth sol-do, and the ascending minor second ti—do

keynote.

sufficiently

ti

a little, anticipating, as

do not employ such small numbers.

mate the values given here

However, they approxi-

for just intonation and can

presumably be

described similarly.)

The

44

chief chord in the key of

C major

is

C, E, G, which occurs not

THE INTERVAL
only with

C

as

the bass but in

two other

E (or mi)

inversion, E, G, C, the

inversion, G, C, E, the chord
sol

become magnet

Mi

"pulls" re

it

stronger ending than the
bass

more used

is

weaker than

tones,

and fa to
in

is

and

It

would be

do,

since the

(or

sol).

Hence, mi and

but effective nontheless.

first

Of

the two, mi makes a

inversion with mi as

music than the second inversion.

for this reason that mi needs little or

who had

G

"pulls" la.

sol

sol,

the bass, and in the second

based on

is

In the first

positions as well.

It is

its

apparently

no resolution.

interesting to speculate about the reactions of people

heard only music in minor keys. The expectation would be

that since me

(E\} )

would now replace

mi as the

much-heard

bass of

the chief chord, the ascending major second (re—mi) and the descending minor second (fa—mi)

there

would no longer display marked finality. But

probability that people will ever be so tonally confined.

is little

Tonality
At

this

point

it

how

reasonable to ask

is

having a marked tonic.

any music can escape

We note the modernist's

interest in atonality

we meet much enthusiasm for polytonality, which the layman
may erroneously regard as another form of atonality. We are also
and

aware that the Oriental, although perceiving
music,

fails

to sense the tonalities of

tonalities are really there,

The

why

into the music.
is

in his

our Western music.

own
If

the

does he not perceive them?

Oriental's lack of sensitivity for our tonics simply under-

scores the point that tonality

he

finality

is,

in the last analysis,

subjective matter.

It is a

The

what one puts

Oriental, unfamiliar as

with the Western music system, needs to hear our resolutions

over a considerable period of time before he

our expectancies. But
with us even

as

we,

after a

is

equipped to appreciate

time he can learn to share experiences

in time, can learn to react "properly" to the

4?

THE INTERVAL
nuances of some of the Eastern systems.

Musical sensitivities are

culturally (but not racially) bound.

music Schonberg tried to

In creating his earlier atonal, or keyless,

would be emphasized above

insure that no tone

acquire keynote status.

its

neighbors and so

His method for achieving this atonality

barred the repetition of any one of the chromatic scale's twelve
tones until the other eleven had

hoped,

on the
of

all

been played. As Schonberg had

this self-imposed rule yielded
first

music which seemed,

few hearings, to possess no
Yet many persons

finality.

tonality;

have

it

saturated

at least

led to no feelings

with

themselves

Schonberg' s music to the point where they have begun to read a

modicum

of tonality into his music.

SchonberCT thought,

is

a relative

Atonality, regardless of

matter after

what

all.

Milhaud and others have for some time been composing polytonal
music. Although to

many

it

sounds keyless, from two to six keys are

functioning at once in the several melodies of the musical fabric. ^^

Attention to any one melodic line will disclose an obvious tonic. Yet
the lay listener normally focuses his attention on the complex, not on
a single strand, so that to

Although
it is

more

we

him

the music

is

virtually atonal.

speak somewhat carelessly of the evolution of music,

accurate to talk simply in terms of cultural changes.

know that "primitives"

the world over tend to have weakly structured

music systems and that they probably have somewhat

we

for

Western

tonality. ^3

Of our own

early

its

gradually

disappeared

until

only

we know

little

music

But over the years the
the

major and

iTiinor

remained.

Late in the sixteenth century the stage was set for strong

tonality as

we now know

it.

Tonality

is still

endeavors of the atonalists, but whether
centuries hence in

46

than

music system could be described with reasonable

accuracy, polymodality was already in vogue.

modes

less feeling

By the time the records of the Western world were

or nothing.
such that

We

its

present strength

is

it

with us

in spite of the

will be with us several

anyone's guess.

THE

I

RVAL

N TE

Consonance and Dissonance
Textbooks on harmony
fifth,

state flatly that the octave, the fourth, the

the thirds, and the sixths are concordant or consonant, Avhile

the seconds, sevenths, and

all

diminished and augmented intervals

are discordant or dissonant. These books, however, have

nothing to offer the student

who

quite properly asks what

little

or

makes the

minor seventh dissonant or the minor third consonant. The student

may know

that he prefers the dissonance of the

concordance of the minor

third. ^4

detective can generally find

He may

to the

also realize that a musical

some musical context

any dissonance will seem highly agreeable.
traditional

minor seventh

It

in

is

which almost

clear that the

conceptions of consonance and dissonance do not so

and that there can, in

readily apply to the present,

fact,

be no

unchanging definition of either.

Among

the rationalizations offered for the consonance-dissonance

classification, the

most accepted, perhaps because of the prestige of

the theorist, has been that proposed by Helmholtz.^5
this scientist, the

According to

consonant intervals are those whose fundamentals

and/ or overtones are free from the roughness caused by beats. ^^

Smoothness, then,

is

Krueger has offered

a similar

It is

remarkable

numerous

the Helmholtzian criterion of consonance.

how

refutations.

theory of beating difference-tones. ^7

these theories have persisted in the face of

One need

only arrange for one of the two

tones of the interval to be piped to the
right to see that

when

all

left

ear and the other to the

psychological effects are largely unchanged even

the possibility of air-borne beats has been eliminated.

More-

over, no beats can be created by the sounding of successively played
intervals.

Yet even Helmholtz

classified these latter just as

simultaneously sounded ones. Here

an interval without beats

same

interval with beats

is
is

branded

is

he did the

an example of name magic, for

as

dissonant simply because the

dissonant.

47

THE INTERVAL
Stumpf's theory effusion can be objected to on similar grounds, ^^

For when he

consonant interval tends to fuse into a

says that the

which

unitary impression

defies

he

analysis,

is

obviously

quite

speaking solely of the simultaneously sounded interval.

Successively

played intervals can be easily separated into their component tones

by the musically untrained
always certain that he

is

as

well as the trained, and the listener

hearing two tones.

is

But with the simulta-

neously sounded interval, with the well-fused octave particularly,
there

A

may be moments

of doubt for

many

persons,

theory stressing the internal structure of the interval was pro-

posed by Lipps.^9 According to

his view, the

consonant intervals are

those with simple ratios, and the dissonances those with the

complex

ratios.

Thus, the consonant

fifth

has the simple ratio of

and the dissonant seventh the more complex

There are many

most

serious, perhaps,

is

its

failure

2

:

3

8:15:.

with Lipps's theory.

difficulties

more

to cover

One

of the

the fact that the

consonant interval can, within limits, be stretched or squeezed,
the ratio

made more complex, without an apparent change

interval quale, 3° If the

perfect

fifth,

it

be rated

will

larger or smaller

it

interval

has been

as

is

still

recognizable

as,

consonant no matter

made or how complex

in the

say,

the

how much

its

ratio has

become.

The hypothesis of

number
interval,

adaptation, that consonance depends

on the

of times an individual, or his ancestors, has heard a given
has been supported

Although the two

men

both by Ogden3^ and by Moore, 3^

differ in the details of their theories, they

agree in employing in an explanatory sense the once popular but

now

discarded biological doctrine of the inheritance of acquired characteristics, 33

Hence,

we

shall

not consider their ideas further except

to note that they have both suaaested the possibility that consonance
is

not absolute, fixed once and for

all.

If

such a suggestion were

followed, the theorists might then go a step forward, drop the

48

THE INTERVAL
consonance-dissonance dichotomy, and replace

it

with

a

continuum.

This would indeed be a gain for musical theory.

So

far

we

have considered theories which have identified con-

sonance with smoothness, with fusion, with simplicity of interval
ratio,

and with stage of cultural and individual adaptation. These four

be so dissimilar that one wonders whether their

criteria appear to

proponents had the same phenomenon in mind. That they probably
did not has been quite well demonstrated by several studies in

persons have been asked to judge intervals

first in

which

terms of smooth-

then for blending, and later on for fusion. 34 Three quite

ness,

judgments emerge. They are so

different sets of

that the early Seashore Sense of Consonance Test,
for judgments

on

all

different, in fact,

whose

directions asked

three criteria at once, proved quite unworkable

and was abandoned. 35 The

became hopelessly confused

test subjects

and ended by rating the intervals in terms of their agreeableness.
In their studies of consonance, the theorists of the past largely

ignored the fact that the

human organism

capable of learning.

is

Inherent in their theories was the assumption that a given interval

should

call forth identical

responses in

extent of their musical training. But
in the study of

as

all

people regardless of the

Cazden

says:

"The

consonance and dissonance dissolve

as

difficulties

soon

as

we

realize that these qualities are not inherent in perception as such but

are learned responses, adaptations to an existing pattern of the social

group. "3^

A

successively sounded major third

A

a simultaneously played third.

different

from

a

major third in

third has a different effect

A

major third

is

on

a

a

by

a

as a

perceptually different from

major third

in a

whole-tone composition.

not an abstraction.

It is

own

is

A

quite

major

composed of two tones

after the other,

part of this composition or that, and

person of our

Bach work

Hottentot and a European theatregoer,

which may be played together or one
played

is

which may be

which may be heard

or some other culture. 37 But to speak of the

49

THE INTERVAL
major third (or of any other interval)

more
were

consonance

is

do no

to

Musical science would be the better

than parrot a dogma.
this

as a

concept dropped from the scholarly literature.

Summary
The music student knows

that all but the tone-deaf can readily

learn to recognize the several intervals of the diatonic and chromatic

know

But what he may not

scales.

that each interval span can,

is

within certain limits, be stretched or squeezed without harm to
distinctive interval quality.

only one facet

The

in the

He

will find that interval uniqueness

composite of psychological

effects

register of the interval, the rate and loudness of

timbre of the instrument on which

harmonic contours

what each
abstraction

What

is

it

in

isolation

of real importance

Hence, the interval

as

an

only slight musical significance.

has
is

playing, the

its

have their part in creating

all

listener has learned to want.

or

is

on the listener.

played, and the melodic and

it is

helps to form

its

some

the interval in

specific musical

context.

The simultaneous playing of two tones forms, by
simultaneous interval, and the sounding of

When

another a successive interval.
alternated yet are

The

trill differs

still

perceived

as

the

two

first

The

trill,

two tones are

tones, a

then,

is

a

one tone and then

trill is

from the vibrato and the tremolo

are heard as single pitches.

definition,

rapidly

produced.

in that the latter

a successive interval,

while the vibrato and the tremolo are but ornamented single tones.

Although composed of two
felt to possess a definite

to

easily localized tones,

pitch level.

"melody hunt" among the higher

more

likely to give the interval

its

If

voices, the top

tone which provides the pitch.
50

it

is

the listener has been trained

pitch location.

has been attending to lower registers

each interval

will

boundary tone

But

if

is

the listener

be the lower boundary

THE INTERVAL
Intervals

possess

number

a

of perceptual

characteristics,

but

although certain intervals are traditionally labeled "major" or "minor,"

them

major or minor

no one

of

only

phenomena of chords and modes.

as

In

displays

Western music

a

number

Such

effects.

effects

of intervals are regarded as restful, as

yielding the effect of finality, whereas others are looked

unresolved.

appear

upon

as

Certain theorists tend to be impressed by the fact that,

other things being equal,

it is

the interval of simple ratio that calls

forth the greatest feeling of finality and rest in persons steeped in

Western music. These men regard

finality effects as physiologically

engendered even though members of other cultures often
the well-resolved intervals of the
sociopsychologically
their
issue

is still

explanations
is

minded

main explanatory
unclear.

may be

West

be

restful.

it is

a fact that

Other, more

At the moment, however, the

formalists 3^ and those given to biological

correct in their belief that the

natively geared to feel the simple-ratio intervals as

But

to find

theorists have offered habituation as

principle.

The

to

fail

human organism
the more restful.

continued experience with a so-called unresolved

interval will lead to a

growing feeling of rest and

music becomes familiar

finality.

Even atonal

in time.

Most theorists and composers are beginning to see that an interval
can be evaluated only in terms of

its

intervals consonant or dissonant

creativity and appreciation.

when

it

No

is

context and that labeling certain
a

stumbling block to musical

interval

is,

per

se,

unmusical even

cannot be formed from diatonic or chromatic notes.

since the listener of any culture

must be taught

is

to

some degree

a traditionalist,

But

he

to like the rarely used and the unfamiliar, and these

must be presented with caution. Indeed, music history

is

in large

part the story of the handling of innovations, of the gradual acceptance
of what was once musically unacceptable.

5-1

4-2

THE INTER VA L
Notes
i]

A number

of the

phenomena we

shall

be describing concern chords and melodies

well as intervals and could quite logically have been treated in other chapters.

as

The reader

must forgive the rather arbitrary decision to consider them here.
2]

E.

M. Edmunds and M.

Intervals," Amer. J. Psychol.,
3]

"The Phenomenological Description of Musical

E. Smith,

34 (i923):287-29i.

O. Ortmann, "On the Melodic Relativity of Tones,"

Monog., 3S, No.

Psychol.

i

(1926): 1-47.
4]

C. V. Maltzew, "Das Erkennen sukzessiv gegebener musikalischer Intervalle in den

aussern Tonregionen," Zsch.f. Psychol., 64 (191 3): 161—25^7.
S]

C. E. Seashore, "The Vocal Trill," Mus. Ed.

6]

C. Stumpf, Tonpsychologie, Leipzig, Hirzel, 1883, 1890.

7]

H.

J.

8]

C.

W.

29,

No.

3

(1943): 40.

Watt, The Psychology of Sound, Cambridge, Cambridge U. Press, 19 17.
Valentine, "The Aesthetic Appreciation of Musical Intervals

Children and Adults,"
9]

J.,

P. R. Farnsworth,

among School

6 (191 3): 190-216.

Brit. J. Psychol.,

"Notes on the Pitch of a Combination of Tones,"

(1924): 82—85^; "The Pitch of a Combination of Tones," Amer.

Brit. J. Psjchol.,

Psychol.,

J.

No.

5;,

25
3

(1938): S3^~Si910]

C. A. Alchin, Applied Harmony, Los Angeles, published by the author, 192

11]

P. R. Farnsworth,

"The

Amer. J. Psjchol., 37 (1926):
12]

L.

A

1

on Ending Preferences

in

i.

Melodies,"

16-122.

demonstrates the

finality

effects

of falling inflection

Kaiser, "Contribution to the Psychologic and Linguistic

Psychol.,

13]

study which

Effect of Repetition

is

by

that

Value of Melody," Acta

9 (i95'3): 288-293.

K. E. Zener, "The Perception of Finality in Simple Tonal Sequences

as

Determined

by Pitch," Doctoral Thesis, Harvard U., 1926.
14]

A good

English language account of the T. Lipps views can be found in volume

2

of

Psychology Classics, K. Dunlap, ed., Baltimore, Williams and Wilkins, 1926.
i^]

M. Meyer, "Elements

of a Psychological Theory of Melody," Psychol. Rev., 7 (1900):
in the Psychology of Music," Amer. J. Psychol., 14 (1903):

241-273; "Experimental Studies

4^6-478.
16]

P.

R. Farnsworth, "Atonic Endings in Melodies," Amer. J. Psychol., 36 (192^):

394-400.
17]

P. R. Farnsworth, "Further Data

the Psychology of
18]

J.

Tone and Music,"

Concerning the Lipps-Meyer Law"

Genet. Psychol. Monog., IS,

Handschin, in Der Toncharakter:

Atlantis Verlag,

1948, asserts that

if

eine

Einjuhrung in die

i

in "Studies in

(1934): 40-44.

Tonpsychologie,

ZiJrich,

the tones of the scale are arranged in an order

corresponding to the number of fifths they are removed from

S2

No.

F,

then the smaller the order

THE INTERVAL
number

more masculine

the

"A Preliminary Study of

R. UpdegrafF,

19]

Acad.

the Nature of Finality in Melody," Proc.

Gestalt explanation of the

suggests the

power of the symbol

2

well-known Law of Pragnanz, which holds

objects in the simplest arrangement possible.

among

the scale symbols,

See

keyed to characteristics of the
in

A

"Theory of Tonality,"

than a matter of mores, see A.

2 is

the smallest symbol to appear

Teach. Nat. Assoc. Proc. 1949,

43

I.

of the inner ear, has been offered by

J. Gen. Psychol.,

who

formation

as

Ser. (195-1):

203-206. E. Franklin has recently suggested

movement

Actually 9 to

division by

i

.

The

essential nature,

be subsumed under one

when

"applied to melody

of the bass or the implied bass." (Tonality as a

Basisjor the Study of Musical Talent, Goteborg,
21]

Sweden, Gumperts Forlag, 1956,

however,

is

2

See Milhaud's Third Sjmphonj for an example of six-key polytonality.

23]

Most American Indian music, however, shows considerable

McAllester, Enemy Way Music, Cambridge, Mass., Peabody
P. R.

Farnsworth and C.

]. Appl. Psychol., 12

H. L.

2^]

191

p. 48.)

not altered by multiplication or

22]

24]

more

is

Elkus, "Tonal Centers and Central Modalities," Mus.

that the Lipps-Meyer law functions only

subordinate to the

For the

3J (1947): 169-176.

believes that the Lipps-Meyer effect

that the principles so far suggested to explain finality effects can
is

who

biological explanation of tonality, this time

membrane

basilar

views of a biologically minded musician

His notion

L. Mursell

J.

that persons tend to perceive

Mursell 's "Psychology and the Problem of the

J.

Scale," Music. Quart., 32 (1946): £64.-^j^.

H. Wunderlich

Since the

comes from

furnishes the simplest possible arrangement and so provides

it

the most satisfying of endings.

rule.

la.

23 (1926): 279-282.

Sci.,

A

20]

Handschin has apparently

be the character of the tone.

will

and restfulness with masculinity.

identified stability

F.

F.

See D. P.

tonality.

Museum, 19^4.

Voegelin, "Dyad Preferences at Different Intensities,"

(1928), 148-151.

Helmholtz, On

the Sensations of Tone, Ellis, trans.,

N.Y., Longmans, Green,

2.

When two

26]

tones which are very close to each other in pitch are sounded together,

the amplitude of the resulting sound
eliciting a

throbbing or beating effect.

is

periodically

augmented and decreased, thus

For example, the sounding of two tones whose

frequencies are 1000 and looi d.v. will yield one beat per second.
beats

is

over 20, the individual beats are not heard; the effect

When, under

certain conditions,

two tones with,

are sounded, a difference tone of 700 cycles

ment of instruments which

say,

may be

is

If

the

Until recently

were relying on beat phenomena to help them tune
that they depend rather on apparent pitch. See J.

Before the develop-

it

made

use of

was assumed that musicians, too,
But

their instruments.
F.

of

frequencies of 8000 and 8700 d.v.

distinctly heard.

give visual pictures of sound waves, piano-tuners

beat counts in adjusting piano strings.

number

merely one of roughness.

it

now appears

Corso, "Unison Tuning of Musical

Instruments," J. Acoust. Soc. Amer., 26 (i^^^.): 746-750.

Krueger,

Differenztone und Konsonanz," Arch. Ges. Psychol.,

27]

F.

28]

C. Stumpf, Tonpsjchologie, Leipzig, Hirzel, 1883, 1890.

"

1

(1903): 205—27^.

5"3

THE INTERVAL
29]

T. Lipps, Psychological

Wilkins, 1926.

by D. B.
Irvine

Irvine,

would

Classics,

What might be
"Toward

a

vol.

regarded

2,

K. Dunlap, ed., Baltimore, Williams and

as a variant

of the Lippsian theory

is

that

proposed

theory of intervals," J. Acoust. Soc. Amer., IJ (1946): 350-3^^.

classify intervals

by families on the

basis of the length of the

composite wave

scheme, the fourth (3:4) and the major sixth (3: j) would belong to one
family, the major third (4: ^) and the ninth (4:9) to another, etc. In other words, all
intervals whose ratios are as 3 to something would be grouped into one classification, 4

form.

By

this

to something into another, and £ to something into
30]

J.

Peterson and F.

W.

Musical Intervals," Amer.

View of Consonance,"
31]

32]

ij,
33]

still

another.

Smith, "The Range and Modifiability of Consonance in Certain

J.

PsjchoL, 42 (1930):

Psjchol. Rev.,

^61-^72;

J.

Peterson,

R. M. Ogden, Hearing, N.Y., Harcourt, Brace, 1924.

H. T. Moore, "The Genetic Aspect of Consonance and Dissonance,"

No.

2

Psychol.

Monog.,

(1914): 1-68.

For an excellent refutation of genetic theories of consonance see "An Experimental

Test of the Genetic Theory of Consonance" by E. G. Bugg and A.
Psychol.,

34]

"A Functional

32 (192^): 17-33.

E.

S.

Thompson,

J. Gen.

4J (19^2): 71-90.
G. Bugg, "An Experimental Study of Factors Influencing Consonance Judgments,"

Psjchol. Monog., 4s,

No.

2

(1933);

C

P. Heinlein,

"An Experimental Study

of the Sea-

shore Consonance Test," J. Exp. Psjchol., 8 (i925'): 408—433; C. P. Heinlein, "Critique
of the Seashore Consonance Test," Psjchol. Rev., 36 (1929): 524-^43.
35']

C. E. Seashore, Manual of Instructions and Interpretations for Measures of Musical Talent,

N.Y., Columbia Graphophone, 19 19.
36]

N. Cazden, "Musical Consonance and Dissonance:

(194^): 3-1
37]

R.

W.

A

Cultural Criterion," J. Aesth.,

4

1-

Lundin, "Toward a Cultural Theory of Consonance,"

y. Psychol.,

28 (1947):

45-4938]

C. C. Pratt,

known through his earlier publications as
now espouses what might be

the role of learning in the arts,

a formalist

Although Pratt backs away from cultural relativism and approaches

his

physical science rather than a social science bias, his theoretical position

to be in complete opposition to that taken in this

Judgments," J.

54-

Aesth.,

IS [1956]: i-ii).

who

plays

down

called contextual relativism.

book ("The

problems with

a

would not appear

Stability of Aesthetic

CHAPTER FOUR

Melody

/vLTHOUGH

usually present a

layman will

somewhat

feel that

larger configuration of tones before the

music

as

such really

At

exists.

musically unsophisticated the basic element of music

tones he terms a tune or an

He

one must

intervals have great musical significance,

air.

what he can

It is

tends to regard most other musical

least for the
is

recall

phenomena

as

a series of

and name.
the tune's

embellishments. This chapter will consider in some detail the tune,

what musicians
Melody,
define.

call

like so

The

the melody.

many other concepts

best general definition

successively sounded intervals and
tion.

is

in music,

that a

is

very difficult to

melody

must have some

is

made up

of

sort of organiza-

But the nature of that organization has puzzled both the

musical theorists and the experimentalists.
frequent statement that a melody

optimum
validity;

variety within unity.

yet

it

tells

us too

is

Such

little.

In the literature

a tonal

may have some
need to know how

a description

We

still

entirely in terms

Certain tonal sequences, they say, are by their very

nature properly unified and
than a few formalists

and optimum.

the

sequence displaying

optimum unity and variety can be identified.^
The formalists attempt to state their definitions
of tonal stimuli.

is

who

show optimum

variety.

feel that intuition tells

But the majority

fall

There are more

them what

is

proper

back on the practices of their

S5

M ELODY
Only what

favorite composers.

Brahms regarded

as

Beethoven, or perhaps a

a Bach, a

properly melodic

can

be accepted.

When

These

formalists

would

reminded

that musical practices are in the process of continual

freeze music at one of

its

periods.

they are

change, they either hold firmly to the rules followed by the earlier
elite

and frown upon the modernists, or

shift

models with the

thought that absolute principles are only gradually revealed. True,
absolutes are implicit in the tonal stimuli and so are fixed and eternal.

But

man must

search

them out with the reward of only

partial

success..

The

relativistic position, the

one accepted by most

favors a definition in terms of
listener

hearing

comes
it

it

learning.^

as

it

which brings
changes,

For

all

its

course, will

of us live in a social organiza-

to our attention certain sequences and ignores, if

sanctions change.

But culture

is

never

static.

Hence, what was once regarded

properly unified and optimally varied

hackneyed, and what once had too

on

holds that a

Not every sequence, of

does not actively frown upon, others.

As

It

to regard a sequence as forming a unity only through

over and over again.

have the chance of repetition.
tion

human

social scientists,

much

may

later

variety

be thought of

may

as

eventually take

unity.

Many

years ago Emerson, employing intervals smaller than our

semitones, constructed what might be called microscopic melodies.

The sequences seemed

to possess little tonality as the musician of the

West knows the term. Yet Emerson's

subjects learned to regard

these unusual patterns as unified and to develop certain expectancies

Emerson had

concerning them.

in effect established a tiny musical

subculture.

A

melody, then, seems best defined

personal thing,

a

in

terms of learning.

56

a

matter of expectancy. Theoretically speaking, any

sequence of tones could conceivably constitute
culture group.

It is

But

at

a

any one time and place only

melody

for

a fraction

some
of the

MELODY
many

possible will receive official approval.

nonconformist

will

patterns until, for
to

which the

persist

him

experimenting with unapproved

become melodies. The extent

at least, these

radical can convince others that they should accept

these sequences

is

a

mark of

shown

has

his greatness.

and Learning

Principles of Attention

Ortmann

in

In spite of this, the

laws of primacy, recency,

that the

and

emphasis, which educational psychologists have found so helpful,
also

hold for melodies. 5

First

and

notes he found to stand out

last

prominently. They often become focal centers for the melody and

The

aid the listener in his quest for unity and coherence.

were

low^est notes

also seen as attention-getting.

highest and

Moreover, any one

of the remaining notes of the melody could, he found, be
equally or even

emphasizing

it

Ortmann' s

in

more prominent by
some other way.
can

observations

giving

easily

be

it

more

loudness or

by

verified

made

analyzing

responses to Seashore's Measure of Tonal Memory.^ In this test threeto five-note sequences are played twice,

with one of the notes

changed on the second rendition. The task

to recognize the change.

Now

it

be the

is

might be guessed that the three-note melodies would always

remember and

easiest to

the problem

is

not so simple.

the five-note the most difficult.

For the

difficulty in recall

is

But

in part a

function of the prominence of the note which has been altered.
Altering the

produce

first

a radical

number of
The law

or the

last

tone, or the highest or the lowest,

may

change in the tonal configuration, regardless of the

tones in the sequence.
of frequency

is

one of the most important principles

melody formation. The themes of the

some new and unfamiliar idiom seem
they are heard a

number

of times.

radical

who

is

composing

in
in

tuneless, i.e., unmelodic, until

The layman who

is

forced to listen

57

MELODY
to such material

may lament

the good old-fashioned melodic music

of the past, without realizing that

music

his great-grandfather,

on

some

of what he

now

calls

melodic

hearing, described as tuneless.

first

Repeated hearing over the years has led to acceptance and, with
the feeling that this kind of melody

now possesses

it,

adequate unity and

coherence.

Many American laymen and psychologists are hard put at times
understand why musical theorists seem so blind to the importance

to
to

their art of the law of frequency, ignoring so completely the function

of learning.

The explanation appears

to be that music theory stems

pretty largely from European philosophy, which was long dominated

by the great formalist Kant. This or that

is

so, said the formalists,

because inherent in the stimulus conjiguration are the qualities that
it

this

make

way. The formalists ignore the possibility that reasons may

lie

frequency of association, in cultural inheritance.

in

The present author does not hold
feels that

many,

if

He

to the formalistic position.

not the majority, of explanations necessary for

understanding the phenomena of music must be looked for in the
habits of the listener, in

culture history.

,

what he has learned from

and

his personal

,

Melody and Pitch Level

Now

and again one may hear of

a small child

who,

complain that

"Mama

his

customary evening

my

song tonight." Mama, of course, will

same

as

on previous

lullaby, will

nights.

And

a pitch level slightly higher or

so

it

insist that

after hearing

didn't sing

the song was the

was, except that

it

was sung

lower than the usual one.

For

child and for a tiny fraction of the population, pitch level

important part of melody.

440

d.v. will for this

do

is

pitched

at,

this

is

an

say,

extremely small number of persons be psycho-

logically different if do

^8

A melody whose

at

is

perceptibly raised or lowered.

MELODY
There was

a time

when most

of the authorities considered the

abihty to identify pitch level, termed "absolute" or "positive" pitch,
the resultant of

some Mendelian principle of inheritance and an

infallible

index of musical precocity.

not quite

as

But the

facts of the case are

the early theorists described them.^

no person can identify accurately by

We now

know

letter every tone given

that

him or

can sing or play correctly any demanded pitch. Varying with a

number

of circumstances, his accuracy will most probably be better

for tones

whose timbre he knows

best.

It

will, for

for piano tones and poorer for the purer tones

some laboratory instrument. 9
range, in the registers

It

example, be good

from

a tuning fork or

will be better for tones in the middle

most used

in music.

It

will be poorer for

black than for white keys.

A

small boy of five studied in the Stanford Psychological Labora-

tory began, while

still

in kindergarten, to notice the pitch level of

mother's songs
o and to "correct" her when her Jo's were as much
a semitone away from their accustomed pitch level. At this age,

his
as

however, he could not make such

fine discriminations

home instrument. But
when tested on his own

tones, even with those of his

years he

showed

great skill

another year's time he could do
tory piano.

Still later, his skill

clarinet tones.

Here we

growing experience with

see a

as

with piano
about two

in

piano, and in

well on the very different labora-

became phenomenal with

growth

violin and

in pitch-level skills paralleling

a variety of timbres.

more skill in
minded persons who

Persons claiming absolute pitch generally display
recognizing pitch level than do other musically

make no such claims. Yet

the overlap in ability between those willing

to claim the ability and the

more modest

the high scorers often decrease in
vastly

improve their

status

skill.

is

large. ^°

Without practice

Those lower

in ability can

with assiduous practice."

After hearing a pitch of 2^6 d.v. frequency, most of us could play

or sing a gii d.v. pitch with reasonable accuracy.

Or

if

given

some
^9

MELODY
kinesthetic cue,

if

can sing, some of us could figure, so to speak, where ^i

we

But

definitely

be reacting

in

would need some sort of reference

2

should be.

point.

We would

terms of relative, not absolute, pitch, or so said the early

on the other hand, there

In absolute pitch,

writers.

we

allowed to probe for the lowest or highest note

no need for

is

auditory or kinesthetic reference points, for the reactions are allegedly
"intuitive"; the faster they are, the

The reader may
biological

more

accurate they will be.

be convinced that absolute pitch

still

relative pitch,

that the

two

matter of

However, the evidence can even more

inheritance.^^

showing either that absolute pitch

readily be interpreted as

extreme

is a

is

but

one end of the relative-pitch continuum, or
while perhaps not completely identical, are

abilities,

highly correlated and are both dependent on musical experience. ^3
It is

a fact familiar to all psychologists that in learning a

an individual at

first

makes use of

motor

skill

of cues or crutches. ^4 As

a variety

the skill improves, less and less attention will be paid these cues.

By

the time the person has achieved real mastery over his task, he can

and does
will
its

be

likely not only to

He

quality.

tuitive" fashion.

Some

slow

does his best

Thus

it is

his
if

he

if

At

A

first

he found

After

position.

humming

this

it

note

he behaves

necessary to
as a

many weeks

disappeared, but for

humming.

Later, this

immediately or the

A

use them, he

in

an unthinking, "in-

with the learning of absolute pitch.

(440 d.v.) to the point of

manage, taking

tries to

performance but seriously to harm

years ago the author developed his

nizing violin
error.

In fact,

for2;et his earlier aids.

need

own
less

hum

accuracy in recog-

than an eighth-tone

the lowest note he could

point of reference to "figure" the

A

of effort, the need for the audible
a

time he

still

also vanished

needed to imagine the

and the

string of the violin could

A

could be sung

be tuned without

outside reference.
It

has been said that

While
60

this

many

"primitives" possess absolute pitch.

claim has not been verified,

it is

reasonable to suppose

MELODY
that a person

whose

were confined

tonal experiences

to a do of

constant pitch would rather quickly develop a relatively good feel
Certainly better pitch-level responses should be

for pitch level.

expected in an area where

all

persons used only French pitch

more

(p. 17)

than in another area where a variety of standard pitches was used.

Many German
in

schools do not teach the movable- Jo system so

common

America. They follow the "tone-word" method which, by assigning

a singable

nearly

name

to each tone of the chromatic scale, gives a

absolute

frame of reference.

make fewer

children so taught

So

far

we

^5

not surprising that

is

errors in pitch level.

have not delimited the term absolute pitch.

would reserve

it

Seashore

for abilities with errors of a tenth of a semitone

(10 cents) or less. ^^ Bachem's criterion

other contemporary researchers allow

No

It

more

is

almost

a far larger

census of the general population has ever been

percentage enjoying absolute pitch.
obvious that the figures can be

made

If

one

is

But

as stringent. ^7

margin of error.

made

to find the

ever undertaken

large or small

it is

depending on the

narrowness of the criterion adopted for absolute pitch.
to conclude that only a person extremely interested in

It is safe

who

pitch level or

melody has

its

has been indoctrinated with the notion that each

"proper" key

(p.

86) will be greatly upset by melody

modulations of small magnitude. The great majority of us think
musically in terms of intervals, not single tones of fixed pitch. Therefore, unless a
register,

its

melody

is

raised or

lowered to

a relatively

unmusical

basic characteristics will not be greatly affected

by

changes in pitch level.

Melody and Loudness
Melodies with unconventional skips and harmonies are noticeably
less bizarre

attention to

when

played at low intensities.

what otherwise would tend

No

to slip

doubt loudness

calls

by unnoticed. The
61

MELODY
below shows

table

a typical set

of preferences obtained from college

Note

students for intervals based on middle C.^^

that,

by and

large,

the softest renditions are the most preferred.

Relation of Loudness to Interval Preference

3

octave soft

15-5

minor 3rd soft
major 6th loud
major 6th medium

27-S

tritone loud

4

tritone soft

15-5

octave

27-S

5

minor 6th

17

4th

29

minor 7th loud
minor 3rd loud

6

minor 7th

soft

18-5

major 7th

30

major 2nd loud

7-5

major 6th

soft

18-5

minor 6th medium

31-S

major 7th medium

7-5

major

medium

20

Sth

31-S

9

Sth soft

21-5

major 2nd

34-S

minor 7th medium
minor 2nd loud
major 2nd medium

34-S

minor 2nd medium

36

major 7th loud

1

4th soft

2

major 3rd

13

3rcl

14

soft

soft

medium

25-S

4th loud

2S-S

minor 6th loud

medium
medium
soft

medium

10

tritone

21-5

octave loud

11

major 3rd loud

23-5

Sth loud

12

minor 3rd medium

23-5

minor 2nd

If

made

sufficiently

loud,

a

33

soft

soft

pitch in the middle register

may

appreciably change in apparent pitch. While estimates differ, one

good experimentalist claims

minor

third. ^9

to have observed a change as large as a

Ordinarily an increase in the loudness of a low or

middle-register vocal tone tends to increase

tone to decrease the pitch.

its

pitch, of a very high

Soft tones are, in general, flatted.^''

blaring discordance then, may, because of

its

The

extreme loudness, have

pitch relationships slightly different from those the composer in-

tended.

Indeed,

shift in pitch

Much

that

it

is

conceivable that on very rare occasions the

might lead to
is

invalid has

less

discordance.

been written about the control of loudness

in playing the various musical instruments.

the assumption on the part of
infinite

number

way

which the

in

others have
little

62

many piano

There

is,

for instance,

teachers that an almost

of tone qualities can be elicited simply by varying the

shown

fingers strike the piano keys.

that the

But Ortmann^^ and

mechanics of piano action allow for very

tone variation. The few effects that do obtain are

made

possible

MELODY
through

hammer

nothing

else.

velocity, impact and

Most of the

striking the keys in various

As anyone with only
knows, there are

ways are

hammer

and pedal

brought about by

illusory.

a passing acquaintance

at least tw^o,

noises,

allegedly

effects

with the modern piano

and sometimes three, foot pedals.

These are the sustaining or loud pedal, the

and, particularly in

soft,

American-made instruments, the sostenuto. Scores

for pedal effects

like those for intensity generally are in rather a primitive state.

so

it is

Even

surprising that professional pianists are in so little agreement

as to the

"proper" use of the pedals.

research

in

this

who

Heinlein,

did extensive

found marked disagreements among the

area,

performances he examined. ^^ As

a

matter of

fact,

no performer he

studied was able even to duplicate the pedal performance he himself

had made ten minutes
have

little

earlier.

Heinlein found music teachers to

precise knowledge of just what the pedals can and can-

not do.
It

should be noted that performers do not follow their pitch scores
slightly,

and employ a

variety of melodic ornaments. Yet these deviations

from the pitch

exactly.

They

intentionally sharp and

flat

scores are relatively slight. "Inaccuracies" in intensity control, on the

other hand, are far
so crude and
it

is

more

extensive.

knowledge of

no wonder

that the

With

intensity-score indicators

finger and pedal possibilities so meagre,

control of loudness often mirrors the

idiosyncrasies of the performer

more than

does those of the

it

composer.
Because of terminological inadequacies,

is

it

musician to designate a particular loudness. There
ever, a unit of loudness, the phon,
use.

(The number of phons

of looo cycles

is

is

available,

the

how-

be enjoying some

number

of decibels a tone

above the reference intensity

loudness to the tone in question.)

is

for

to

which appears

equal to the

difficult

The phon

when judged
is

equal in

approximately the

smallest increment of loudness that can be noticed under ordinary

63

MELODY
circumstances.

The

below

table

some

gives

idea of the average

loudness of various noises.

Type of Noise

Phons

Airplane engine 10 feet from propeller

120-130

Riveting machine 35 feet away

Pneumatic

drill a

few

feet

102

90-100

away

Conversation

60

Quiet suburban street

40

Quiet whisper

20

The contemporary conductor Leopold Stokow^ski

is

said to inter-

pret his scores as follows:

ppp=

p^

2o phons

pp=4o phons

ff= 8g phons

55 phons

mj^6g

phons

1=75

phons

jJJ=^^ phons

Melody and Timbre
It is

unfortunate that authorities write

minor chord, or

fifth, a

whether played on
violin.

What

a

a

melody

marimba,

a

at

times as though a perfect

will have identical characteristics

harmonica,

a tuba,

or an old

Cremona

they are neglecting are the differences in timbre, the

constellations of partial tones each instrument adds to the fundamental
tones. ^3

The tuning fork and

simplest tones one can

certain organ pipes will yield the purest,

meet outside an

tones are relatively free of overtones.

acoustics laboratory, for their

The

air

columns around them

vibrate largely at only one frequency for each pitch.

instruments, by and large, give very impure tones.

But orchestral

In

some, certain

of the overtones even match the fundamentals in loudness. ^4
Flute tones and soprano voices are relatively pure; tones from soft

horns, soft male voices, pianos and strings are richer,

more complex;

tones from wood-winds (except the flute), loud male voices, and loud
brass (in this order) are

brilliant, cutting, blaring^,

64

more complex and may be described as
or even strident. The tones of the clarinet

still

MELODY
emphasize the uneven-numbered partials and are usually regarded

hollow and

(See also Chap,

nasal, ^5

g, p.

as

Melodies in a low

92.)

register have richer quality than those of high pitch.

Many

of us associate melodies of bagpipe timbre with things

Scottish, the

fife

or

drum with

the military, the oboe family with the

oriental world, and the pipe organ with church.

male

associate the

may

falsetto

whose

lose

with buffoonery,

falsetto

much

of

a serious

we

often

melody sung

in

serious quality. Yet for the Cantonese,

its

employs no female voices, the male

stage

Because

falsetto replaces the

female voice and so w^arrants serious listening.

There are those who
precisely as

Bach on

its

One

really prefer

own

that

composer played

modern organ or

a

orchestra.

their

feel

can only

it.

music should always be played

To

these people

to arrange his

music for the present-day

wonder how many of

Bach on the baroque organ and

these conservatives

how many

However, there

intellectualizing.

a sin to play

it is

is

are victims of

no question but

that

the psychological effects of Bach's polyphony depend in some degree

on the type of organ employed.
It is

a bit disheartening to find that as

one grows older the timbre

of what one hears progressively changes. The

human

ear

becomes

gradually deafer, especially for tones in the highest registers and the

higher overtones cease to exist. The average thirty-year-old hears
well as he did a decade earlier.

almost

as

age

likely

is

as

man

years of

fifty

to be appreciably deafer, particularly for tones of

2048 d.v. and above.

low

But the

By seventy years the deafness may extend

the 1024 d.v. level.

Another way of looking

as

^^

at this

age-decrement

is

to

compare the

hearing of the several age-groups at one pitch level, say at 8192 d.v.
In the

decade from the early twenties to the early thirties there

change. But by age forty there

is

often a ten-decibel loss.

The

is little

testable

loss tends to

grow

more than 40

decibels by age sixty. ^7 These loudness losses have little

to approximately

2

^ decibels

by age

fifty

and to

65

MELODY
relevance to the hearing of the melody's fundamentals, but they bar
the older person from hearing the full richness of the overtone matrix.

Many

instruments have important resonance areas in the higher

pitch reaches. The partials in these areas are lost to the older listeners.
In consideration of these hearing losses

vv^e

should, perhaps, be

When

considerate toward crotchety old music critics.

critic maintains that the tones of a certain orchestra

performer are not so

full

and rich

correct; for to his aging ears the tonal mass truly

he

to realize

fails

come

that the time has

is

younger colleagues the evaluation of tonal

We

saw^ earlier (p. 9) that the

playing of old

Cremona

one such

or virtuoso

they once w^ere he

as

for

is

him

more

is

quite

less rich.

What

to leave to his

effects.

timbre effects elicited from the

violins are in part illusory.

There are other

situations w^here the alleged timbre effects are better attributed to

some other
what

sense than the auditory.

said to

is

one study

it

An

instance of this sort concerns

be the coldness of Heifetz's violin performances.^^ In

was

first

demonstrated that Heifetz

is

indeed regarded

one of the coldest of concert performers. But whenever

his

as

recorded

performance of a particular composition was compared with another's
recording of the same piece

Szigeti

latter's playing that

and Totenberg and Milstein

seemed occasionally

Heifetz coldness

was usually the

is

was

Morini and Elman were thought to be quite

rated as the colder.
definitely colder,

it

less cold.

It

at least as cold.

would appear, then,

that the

probably due to the visual effects of his

posture or lack of
playing looks colder,

facial

we

Since Heifetz's

expression.

imagine that

it

also

Only

stiffer

manner of

sounds colder.

Melody and Sonance
Tone

quality and timbre appear as

musical treatises.

in

most

But, as Metfessel points out, timbre refers only to

instantaneous pictures of the

66

synonymous terms

sound complex. ^9

Under the broader

MELODY
caption of tone quality must also be considered the progression of the

complex
Sonance,

which Metfessel has coined the word "sonance."

for
as

the term

is

now

used, refers to the progressive changes

moment

and fusions which take place within the tone from

moment.

to

Like timbre, sonance furnishes a setting for melody and

can greatly affect

psychological characteristics.

its

The most worked-over

area of sonance

is

melodic ornament described elsewhere

that of the vibrato, a

(Chap.

i).

It

be

will

recalled that in employing the vibrato the singer or instrumentalist
varies his tones periodically

from those of the score.

Sonance

also

appears in nonperiodic and in erratic tone fluctuations. 3°

much impressed by

Seashore has been

never sung or played precisely

as

the fact that a

melody

is

The introduction of the

scored.

vibrato and of glides, and the intentional sharping or flatting of tones
illustrate

how

the performer can stamp his individuality on the

Seashore has generalized from these data to the point of

music.

formulating an aesthetic rule: beauty, he thinks,
deviation from the rigid and regular.

the photograph, which, with

not so highly rated

as

its

He

lies

in artistic

has offered the analogy of

too faithful copying of an object,

is

the less representational painting (p. 142).

But whether or not the deviation principle should be elevated to the
position of an artistic law,

we must

agree that the "horizontal"

impurities of the vibrato and of the erratic and the nonperiodic
fluctuations of tone are, musically speaking, extremely important.

Sonance,
impurities

then,

must be considered along with those

we term

"vertical"

timbre.

Melody and Noise

We

have

seen

characteristics

that

when

it is

a

melody shows

different

psychological

played on instruments of dissimilar timbre.

Noise, too, attaches itself to melody and aids in the creation of most

67
5-2

MELODY
Noise has been defined

musical experiences.

complex, or so irregular, or both, that
heard by

itself. "3^

There

is

no

definite

it

boundary between noise and

tone, for experts can sometimes detect a pitch

seems only

which

layman

to the

conglomerate of unpitched sounds.

a

The rubbing of the bow, the tapping of the
keys3^ or

sound either so

as "a

seems to have no tone when

on the fingerboard of

on the piano

fingers

a violin, the hissing of the breath in

playing the flute, and the plucking sounds necessary to harp-playing
all

serve as excellent examples of noise in music.

belief of some, noise

On

at all costs.

not unpleasant per

is

the contrary,

it is

se,

nor

Contrary to the
is it

to

be avoided

often deliberately sought to give

pleasure.

That beating

effects

add to the fullness of tonal experience can be

observed by listening to the pipe organ. This versatile instrument
assigns

them

more

than one pipe to each pitch but does not achieve for

precisely the

pipe organ

its

same pitch. The beats

in the thinner sounding

sing pipes

audition.

which

felt as

much

missed

and awe by posseshuman threshold of

effect of massiveness

whose frequencies are below the

While these slow

they can be

is

and more exactly tuned electric organ. There

which achieve the

are organs

so created help to give the

characteristic quality, an effect

periodicities cannot be heard as tones,

building vibrations and sometimes heard as faint

rattlings.

The modern

orchestral

composer often

feels the

need for more

noise effects than the traditional instruments can provide.
excite and shock his audience with his use of apparatus

He may

which

yields

rhythmic grunts, siren wails, noises usually associated with the
factory,

and even more unusual sounds.

Henry Cowell,

temporary American composer, has put noise to work
compositions.

piano strings.

massage of the
68

a

con-

in his piano

Plucking sounds arise from a harplike picking of the

An

eerie, wailing effect

strings.

comes from

And, following Dandrieu,

a light stroking or

who

in the early

MELODY
1

700's expressed the cannon's roar by striking the lower harpsichord

notes with his

where the

Cowell has long been playing "tone-clusters"

fist,

fist,

the

of the hand, or even the entire forearm

flat

simultaneously depresses

the keys

all

it

Cowell 's

can encompass.

techniques are especially effective for program music.

Melody and Tempo
It is a matter of common
which

a

melody

is

played

observation that the
often an index of

is

position set to walking speed

slow speed
waltz.

As

a dirge,

we

and

still

shall see in the

an important determinant of
as

major or

Some

as

may

some intermediate speed

a

next chapter, the tempo of a melody

is

its

at

activeness, of whether

fixed in

told to

mind

move back and

deemed proper. The

were

if

college students had

The

subjects

forth a large speed lever

were hearing was

1

at the rate

were

16 quarter-notes to the minute,

what the Aeolian Corporation regarded
fox- trots

reacted to

lever settings given by this group

generally in the neighbourhood of

The

it is

for waltz time. 33

until the playing of the composition they

just

A com-

Duo-Art player-piano, made by the Aeolian

tempo well

were blindfolded and
they

function.

minor.

years ago a

specific

its

well be a march, another at very

another

Corporation, was used in an attempt to learn

one

tempo or speed with

at that

time

as

proper.

usually set at a considerably faster tempo, at

approximately 143.
Further research on dance tempo was carried on six years later by

Lund with

a similar

faster speeds

were by

sample of college students. 34 Lund found that

time considered proper,

this

and i^g for the fox-trot.
leaders recognize

Lund noted

two waltz times,

a

that

many

i

39 for the waltz

dance-orchestra

slower one that approximates

the value uncovered in the earlier study, and the so-called concert or
faster style

whose tempo

is

about the figure his subjects considered
69

MELODY
The

proper.

tempo

fox-trot also has several

variants, of

which two,

the Charleston and the Black Bottom, had considerable popularity for
a time.

Conductors and performers often
develop some particular

effect.

In

alter the

in order to

one movement of the

Symphony (The Funeral March), Beethoven's

Eroica

own marking was

set at

Koussevitzky cut the speed to 74 for his conducting, Beecham

80.

to

tempo

and Toscanini to ^2. Wagner once complained that the

62,

Tannhduser Overture took twelve minutes under his

long

as

own

baton but

as

twenty under certain other conductors.

Music seems to have more than
culties.

Confusion abounds in

this

time, tempo, periodicity, takt, true
defined.

In the

next section

we

its

share of nomenclature

tempo
beat, 35

area

diffi-

where terms such

as

and rhythm are variously

shall discuss the relation

of melody to

rhythm. The reader should be warned that other authors might have
included portions of the discussion on rhythm in this section on

tempo.

But although tempo and rhythm are obviously very closely

related, they are

by no means

identical. 3^

Melody and Rhythm
Rhythm

is

more than the

periodicity resulting from the continuous

repetition of a simple sequence such as do,

re,

mi, fa.

To

elicit

rhythm,

one element from among the four symbols must be emphasized

some way

to

make

stand out from

it

its

fellows.

If

the do alone

in

were

made more intense, if it were held longer than any one of the others,
or of it were somehow made qualitatively unique, say given a very
different timbre, then the sequence would possess what has been
termed objective rhythm.
All three of these

simultaneously.

The

modes of emphasis

pianist, in striking

are used in music, often

more

strongly the

first

note

of a rhythmic pattern, not only makes the tone louder but auto-

70

MELODY
matically alters

its

timbre in

slight

degree

and

also breaks the regularity of his timing

Unwittingly, he

as well.

tarries longer than

he

thinks he does on the stressed tone. 37 In trying, then, to use intensity
as a

means of emphasis the

pianist has also

employed both timbre and

timing to create his rhythms. The organist, of course, cannot so easily
control his intensity relationships and must depend

more on timing

in his attempts to bring about emphasis.

Rhythms serve the

listener

by increasing

his perceptual

span,

dividing up patterns of melodic material so that larger units can be

more

When

readily grasped.

rhythmic emphasis

the

on the

tonic, dominant, etc.,

important positions of the

scale, e.g.,

the tonality structure

made more obvious and

is

anticipations are whetted.

By

on the

is

the listener's

shifting the accent to a tone

which

is

is,

by synco-

pation, the listener's anticipations are strengthened even

more and

normally not accented in a given rhythmic pattern, that

the intensity of the adjacent time-keeping beat

The most obvious
motor

gift

rhythm brings

activity. This activity

movements too

slight to

often occur while jazz music

may reach only

is

played. 39

to eliminate

activity-inducing quality of

rhythm

Simple labor, such

as that

music.

still

on contemplation rather than on

rhythm and endeavored

heavy loads

music

intensified. 3^
is its

invitation to

the covert stage with

be casually observed, or the rhythms may

induce overt nodding, foot tapping, and

stress

to

is

it as
is

other activities, such

The

early church, with

as
its

action, looked askance at

far as possible. 4°

The

earthy,

well demonstrated in

of unskilled

workmen

work

passing

down a line, is made easier because of the highly rhythmic
And although many lay dancers pay little or no

chanties they sing.

attention to the

rhythm of the music

to

which they are dancing, the

more skillful do dance in time with the music and are guided by it.
Combing through the work of the past, Mursell has tentatively
concluded that there are only seven unit groups among the musical
rhythms. 4^

In the table below, the dash designates the emphasized

71

MELODY
element in each

case.

The reader can

readily find illustrations of the

iamb, trochee, dactyl, and probably the amphibrach, although the

much

latter is

illustrates

Lundin

the least frequent.

the

single

offers the

beat,

rumba

as

Unit Groups



— w
— w
v-/

Handel's Dead March from Saul

and Chopin's Opus S3 the tremolo.
an example of the anapest.^^

Musical Rhythms

in

accented,

^

unaccented

Iamb

w

Trochee

v^

Dactyl

1^

1^ —
— w

'u Ky \j

- I

I

I

rhythmic units tends to possess

seems more

Dunlap

is

somber. 43

referring less to

Tremolo

Single Beat

According to the experimentalist Dunlap,

units

Anapest

Amphibrach

a

music with short

joyous quality and that with longer
It

would appear, however,

that

we

have

rhythm than

tempo, and

to

more

already noted that faster music seems

active or major, while

slower tunes appear inore somber or minor. The available evidence
indicates that the affects given

on either
culture

cultural

we do

not rush a corpse to

with measured tread.
funereal.


trochee

melody by rhythm depend very much

conditioning or personal experience.
grave, but carry

its

Hence, rhythms with

from those of

take on a

a

5"

2

have already

slowly and

single beat

may seem

march

flavor.

Other

illustrations of

would be somewhat

commented on

and

7

in the

our

different

the fact that

Western music has

on the rather simple rhythmic patterns

-beat, 3 -beat, 4-beat, and, to a lesser extent, the 6

and

8.

The

occur so rarely that laymen are usually ignorant of their

existence and

fail

to appreciate theiTi. These rarer rhythms do occur

Orient and could enjoy more use

were devised which forced movements
72

it

people of an alien culture.

specialized, so to speak,

the

our

Infantrymen usually favor one or the other foot, so the

may well

associations could be gathered, but they

We

In

in the

West

if

dances or

drills

in these patterns (p. 4).

MELODY
fortunate that the abiUty learned by one set of muscles "crosses

It is

over" in some degree to other

A rhythmic

sets.

one, learned through drill with the right hand,

by the entire organism. The

pattern, say a 5^-beat

is

in actuality learned

hand, either foot, or, in

left

any

fact,

mobile part of the body, can beat out the rhythm. After the pattern
has been thoroughly mastered

upon

a

Such

a superimposition

can be superimposed,

it

is

termed "subjective rhythm."

later in 4-beat can readily

that there

is

siderable

first

do

as

no rhythmic pattern

could not hear a

5^-beat

contact with

rhythm
5^-beat

in

if

the objective rhythm

is

person

when he knows

in the physical stimuli.

unless, of course,

But he

he had had con-

If a

melody

terms of that rhythm.

in

is

possesses a strong and

very weak, the listener

may

supply his

subjective rhythm.

A number

of the early missionaries to Africa brought back the

surprising tale that the natives

seemed poor

in

rhythms and often beat

randomly on their drums. Since drumming was
as

A

-beat rhythm, then in 3-

requested even

familiar objective rhythm, the listening

But

2

music or had been subjected to

laboratory drill with this rhythm.

own

were,

sequence of events which physically has only periodicity.

asked to hear a metronome beat

and

as it

a sort of telegraphy

well as musical behavior and so was a highly skilled activity, such

a report did not

seem very reasonable.

More

careful observations

proved the missionaries to have been quite mistaken. These
studies

showed the rhythmic patterns

to

later

be extremely precise but

too complicated for Western-trained laymen to follow. The African

drummers,

it

patterns, i.e.,

was found, were simultaneously tapping
2

against 3, 3 against 4,

Western music has not

2

against

in

D

against g, etc.

entirely neglected polyrhythms.

be observed, for instance, in MacDowell's
Chopin's Nocturne

3

Flat.

Certain

in several

Tragic

They can

Sonata

or in

modern composers, notably

Henry Cowell, have employed cross- or polyrhythms rather extensively,

and composers of "hot" jazz have traditionally superimposed
73

MELODY
rhythmic cycles of three beats on the fundamental rhythm of two or
four beats. 44 But the majority of conservatories and schools of music
offer

no

how

the student will be able to play these polyrhythms on the

special training for their mastery.

It is

assumed that some-

occasions he finds the need, 45

The schools

that

do teach the handling of polyrhythms employ one

some combination of

of two procedures or

procedure

is

the kinesthetic method, by which the hands (or feet)

are separately trained in

two or more rhythms. This

scheme featured by Jaques-Dalcroze
mies. 4^

The most obvious

these.

The method

is

also

in his

is

supposedly the

dance system of eurhyth-

used by the pianist Arthur Hardcastle,

w^ho on being tested by the author some years ago was found to be

very

A

Hardcastle could tap

skillful,

3 against 7,

and 4 against

7

with

2

against 3,

less

3

against 4, 3 against ^,

than a 10 per cent error.

second approach to the mastery of polyrhythms

oneself the least

common multiple

to be played. For example,

from

I

through

the other on the

i

and

by counting to

(or fraction of this) of the rhythms

in learning to play 2 against 3,

and taps with one hand on the

6

is

i

,

3

and

one counts
g,

and with

4,

Other Hand

One Hand
X

X

1

2

X

3

X

4
X

5

6

Where

When

the multiple becomes large,

playing

counts of the

against £,

3

first

it

can be

split into parts.

one hand taps on the

group of five, on the second and

first

fifth

and fourth

counts of the

second group, and on the third count of the third group. The other

hand
74

taps

on the

first

count of each group of

five.

MELODY
O THER Hand

One H AND
X

X

1

2
3

X

4
5

X

1

X

2
3

4
X

5

X
2

X

3

4
5

At Stanford University there has been devised

a third, or Gestalt,

procedure which appears to have an advantage over the other methods
in that

comphcated patterns can be learned almost

simpler.

The scheme

necessitates

pressing a phonograph record.

the

2

in

2-beat

the

cutting a player-piano roll or

In presenting the polyrhythms, say

against 3, the roll or record

middle C's

as readily as

rhythm and

is

made

to offer a succession of

a series of G's in 3-beat.

The

learner merely listens and attempts to duplicate the C's and G's at

some higher or lower piano

register.

If

the piano roll

is

employed,

the beginner has additional cues from watching the keys as they are
automatically depressed.

Harmony
Singing in unison

is

undoubtedly

and men, or w^omen and

men

as

old as music.

join in singing the

But when boys

same melody, the

make it easier for
The octave span was

natural differences in the lengths of the vocal folds

the

men

to sing an octave

below the

others.

75

MELODY
probably adopted for perceptual reasons

as well.

Its

two tones

fuse

so completely that the musically untutored often imagine the singing
to be

all in

unison. This confusion

less likely to

is

occur with any of

the other intervals.

Another early breakaway from unison singing came with the drone
bass.

Here, one tone

of the melody.

is

continuously sung or played for the duration

In effect, the

drone becomes

a

keynote which adds

greatly to the stability, i.e., to the tonality, of the musical structure.

At times the

or dominant,

sol,

Nowadays the drone

bass

is

is

most

the pipe organ has a place for

it

additionally

employed

as a

drone.

readily heard in bagpipe music, and
in the pedal, so called because the

foot sustains the drone.

Many
to

of the world's music systems went no farther along the road

harmony.

Their major innovations were often in rhythmic

development, an area in which Western music has done relatively
But the West, perhaps during the ninth century, took a logical

little.

next step in developing the organum in which there were
in addition to the octave, voices a fourth

moving

parallel to the

although one with

theme. Here, then,

is

finally

a type of

monotony of

moved

harmonizing,

parallel octaves, fifths,

in opposite directions.

the only intervals officially recognized

At

first

were the same three "perfect"

But since the very mechanics of opposing motion necessarily

ones.

created other intervals, thirds and sixths gradually

came

into vogue.

these innovations, simple parallel motion gradually dropped in

favor to the point

again dared
It is

where

later harmonists proscribed its use.

It

was,

not until the contemporary period that composers once

in fact,

employ

a fact of

become
76

both

drove composers to the scheme of opposing

motions, where two melodies

With

now added,

a fifth above,

little variety.

After several centuries the

and fourths

below and

it

to any considerable extent.

music that the composing habits of any era tend to

rigidly codified.

The

rules take

on the character of taboos

MELODY
which keep

all

but the braver composers from experimentation. Yet

each great composer establishes his
a

few of the

rules

own

school by breaking at least

which have fettered music up

process of breaking old rules and making

new

won

and gradually

a

is

several

Although poly-

homophony, appeared during the Renaissance
dominant position

entirely replaced polyphony.

music

which

in

melodic strands are simultaneously sung or played.
rival,

this

ones, simple opposing

motion gradually broadened into polyphony,
phony's great

By

to his time.

In fact, a

in

Western music,

it

never

good proportion of modern

quite polyphonic in character.

During the heyday of polyphony, any simultaneously sounded
interval or

chord was more or

less a fleeting, fortuitous affair

happened with certain juxtapositions of the several melodic

homophonic

which

lines.

In

became the focus of

writing, however, a single line

attention and the other parts took on a decidedly secondary aspect.

became the

These

latter

effect

was music

intervals

as

clothes, the ornaments, for the melody.

the present-day layman hears

became important

in their

own

right,

it.

The

Chords and

and arguments about

which ones were allowable were heard. But gradually the harmonic
rules changed until, as the reader well

has by

now had

its

knows, almost every chord

day of glory.

Summary
Although in the past
tonal pattern

was taught

that, to

be

a

proper melody,

must have inherent unity and coherence,

that the unity

the listener.

it

is

not in the tonal line at

It is

all

but

is

it

now appears

literally

naturally difficult to feel unity in patterns

virtually impossible to sing or play.

a

read in by

which are

Yet even such tonal contours,

if

heard sufficiently often, become increasingly familiar so that the
hearing of any one of the tones arouses the feeling that this should be

followed by the next tone in the sequence.

It is

this

expectancy,

77

MELODY
then, that characterizes our conception of melody. 47

phenomena which obey the

perceptual

Melodies are

far

more than

principles of learning.

familiar sequences of tones.

persons with "absolute pitch," a melody
characteristics

if

Melodies are

the pitch register

may appear

altered ever so

is

To

the few

to change

little.

its

But for

the great majority of the musical public, melodies can be modulated
freely without

much change

in psychological effect.

Relative and

absolute intensities, on the other hand, are important to almost

Since this

listeners.

is

so, it

is

unfortunate that the

all

staff signs for

loudness variations are extremely crude and that the control of

loudness changes by vocalists and instrumentalists

is

incredibly poor.

For loudness nuances may make just the difference between the
acceptance of a melody or

its

But perhaps in the years to

rejection.

come, musicians will develop symbols which
notation.

It

will permit

more

exact

should be noted that finer units of measurement, e.g.,

phons, already are available to the musician.

Pure tones have no place
features

of tonal

variety

a

vibrato), and noises

—and

Indeed, each musical culture

in music.

impurities

— timbres,

sonances

trains its public in their appreciation.

New timbre effects are accepted slowly and grudgingly.
this

now

(e.g.,

For centuries

conservatism in Western music could be readily excused.
that the innovator can

sources of

modern

The job

largely a

is

But

draw on the almost inexhaustible

electronics there

is

re-

less justification for timidity.

propaganda one of convincing the public that

all

worthwhile timbres were not discovered by the old masters.

The older person
heard them

often imagines that

phonograph he will hear

his radio or

if

he turns up the volume of

his favorite

when he was younger. But

melodies just

as

he

deafness tends to be selective,

with the higher registers affected earlier than the lower. Hence, the
elderly miss
as

having a

much

of the tonal complex and perceive their melodies

less rich

timbre.

in loudness sensitivity.

78

Deafness, then,

is

more

than a weakness

M ELODY
Tempo and rhythm

often cue the Hstener to the melody's function.

With one temporal arrangement a waltz is indicated, with another
a dirge. The proper use of syncopation can whet the auditor's
anticipations. Although certain other cultures have made great use
of rhythmic complications, the West has not. A few courageous
contemporaries have attempted to free their music from the tyranny
of the quarter-note by the use of polyrhythms; but music teachers

have made

little effort

to train their students in the execution

appreciation of such "novelties." They

fail

and

to realize that they are

bypassing a rich source of aesthetic enjoyment.

Slow though the Occident has been
rhythmic materials,

it has

the stage of unison singing, the
basses,

experimenting with

in

led the world in melodic improvisation.

West

slowly

moved

to the use of drone

theorganum, opposed motion, polyphony and

phony with its chordal structures

,

It

From

finally to

homo-

should be noted that music systems

do not necessarily evolve. The music of a people may remain relatively
static, it

may proceed

toward an

from

along one line or another, or

earlier set of forms.

satiation, a goal

we

may

turn back

The composer's task is to keep his public

which can be achieved

In the present chapter

it

in countless fashions.

have concerned ourselves largely with

the anatomical side of music, with

its

flesh

and bones.

We

shall

continue in the next chapter with our discussions of melody, but
there

we

shall

emphasize

its

more dynamic

aspects.

Several facets of

the language problem will interest us.

Notes
i]

G. D. Birkhoff,

in Aesthetic Measure,

Cambridge, Mass., Harvard U. Press, 1933, has

presented a mathematical formula for assessing unity and variety which at

first

glance

seems delightfully straightforward.

But even Birkhoff has been unable to apply

except the simplest materials,

present author found when, in the middle 1930's, he

sent

him

as the

several melodies to assess.

it

to any

Apparently Birkhoff derived his formula from the

practices of composers as described in the

harmony manuals.

79

MELODY
J.

G. Beebe- Center and C. C. Pratt, in "A Test of Birkhoff 's Aesthetic Measure, "J. Gen.
IJ (1937): 339-35^3, have been brave enough to try out the formula with simple

Psychol.

,

Although they expressed themselves

musical materials.

hopeful of the ultimate utility

as

of the Birkhoff scheme they have not published further in this area.

Another formalistic measure of beauty has been proposed by Cyril Burt

How

Psychology of Art" chapter of

D + /\-i

/

Burt's aesthetic index
is

differentiated,

is

|

exp

the

/ is

|

number

in "The
Mind Works, London, Allen and Unwin, 1933.

the

,

where D

is

the

number

of items into which the

of relations integrating the items, and S

is

work

the scope of

apprehension under the usual conditions.
2]

L. L. Thurstone,

3]

L. E.

"The Problem of Melody,"

Music. Quart., 6 (1920):

426-429.

Emerson, "The Feeling- Value of Unmusical Tone-Intervals," Harvard
269-274. The work of Emerson and the studies by

Stud., 2 (1906):

Psjchol.

Max Meyer, "Elements

of a Psychological Theory of Melody," Psjchol. Rev., 7 (1900): 241-273, and by W. V. D.
Bingham, "Studies in Melody," Psjchol. Monog., 12 (191 o): 1-88, are examples of the
excellent early American researches on melody.

H. Werner,

4]

in

"Musical

'Micro-scales'

and

'Micro-melodies'," J.

Psjchol.,

10

(1940): 149-156, has shown that after a sequence has once been accepted as a melody
it

much

can be drastically changed without

intervals proportionately until they

all

O. Ortmann, "On the Melodic

S]

1—47.

J. P. Guilford

See also

loss of identity.

were of microtone

His procedure was to reduce
size.

Short Musical Melodies," J. Exp. Psjchol., 16 (1933): 32-54;

Nelson, "Changes in the Pitch of Tones

Exp. Psjchol.,

6]

J.

P. Guilford

when Melodies Are Repeated," J.

19 (1936): 193-202; "The Pitch of Tones in Melodies
_/.

No.

Relativity of Tones," Psjchol. Monog., 35,

i

(1926):

and R. A. Hilton, "Some Configurational Properties of

as

Compared with

and H. M.

Exp. Psjchol.,

Single Tones,"

20 (1937): 309-335.

C. E. Seashore, D. Lewis, and

J.

G. Saetveit, Manual, Seashore Measures of Musical

Talents,

N.Y., Psychol. Corp., 1956.
7]

With an arrangement

similar to that found in the Seashore Tonal

Wunderlich ("The Recognition Value of the Steps of the Diatonic

Memory

test,

H.

Scale," Amer.J. Psjchol.,

S3 (1940): 579-582) studied the ease of identifying the scale steps, Theja and ti were
re, and la were readily

quite difficult to identify as having been altered while the do,
identified.

See also C. P. Heinlein's

Melodic Configuration

Memory

in

Tonal

"A

Memory

Brief Discussion of the Nature and Function of

with Critical Reference to the Seashore Tonal

Test, "J. Genet. Psjchol., 35 (1928), 45-61 and R. Frances' "Recherches Experi-

mentales sur

la

Perception de

la

Melodie,"

J.

Psjchol.

Norm. Path., 47-Sl

(i9S4):

439-4^78]

For a review of the literature on absolute pitch see D. M. Neu's "A Critical Review of

the Literature on 'Absolute Pitch'," Psjchol. Bull., 44 (1947): 249-266.
9]

10]

B. L. Riker,

No. 193 (1932).

80

"The

L. A. Petran,

Ability to Judge Pitch, "J. Exp. Psjchol.,

"An Experimental Study

36 (1946), 331-346.

of Pitch Recognition," Psjchol. Monog., 42,

M ELODY
1 1]

Building on the early

work

of

M.

F.

Meyer,

"Is the

Memory

of Absolute Pitch

Capable of Development by Training?" Psychol. Rev., 6 (1899), J14-J16, others have
found as he did that by dint of careful training, errors can be reduced to as small a figure
as 33 cents.

See Helen K. Mull's "The Acquisition of Absolute Pitch," Awer. J. Psychol.,
Psjchol. Bull., 3S

36 (192 j): 469-493, and C. H. Wedell's "A Study of Absolute Pitch,"
(1941): ^^47-548.

A. Bachem

12]

pitch

is

Amer.,

is

one of the research

1 1

of this area

who

is still

convinced that absolute

W.

J. Acoust. Sac.

(1940): 434-439; "Time Factors in Relative and Absolute Pitch Determination,"

]. Acoust. Soc. Amer.,

13]

men

an inherent type of behavior. ("The Genesis of Absolute Pitch,"

F.

26 (19^4),

75^1-75^3.)

Oakes, "An Experimental Study of Pitch Naming and Pitch Discrimination

Reactions," J. Genet. Psychol., 86 (i9SS), 237-2^9.
14]

Even when the cues do not directly benefit the performance their presence may give

M. Brammer found

the person confidence. Thus, L.

that the pitch-level scores of several

were no better when they were given the opportunity to tune their own violins
than when the experimenter tuned the instruments for them under their orders. Yet the

violinists

added kinesthetic cues the manipulation of their own

fiddles gave

them increased

their

confidence in their scores. ("Sensory Cues in Pitch Judgment," J. Exp. Psjchol., 41 (1951):

336-340.)
I

£]

Karl Eitz, Das Tonwort, Leipzig, Breitkopf u. Haertel, 1928.

16]

C. E. Seashore, "Acquired or Absolute Pitch," Mus. Ed. J., 26 (1940): 18.

17]

A. Bachem, op.

cit.

The tonal intervals were played on a Duo- Art reproducing piano. When the intensity
levers were set at "soft" the intervals termed "soft" were elicited. Other combinations of
the levers yielded the "medium" and "loud" intensities. See P. R. Farnsworth and
C. F. Voegelin, "Dyad Preferences at Different Intensities," J. Appl. Psjchol., 12 (1928):
18]

148— 1
19]

5^1

H. Fletcher, Newer Concepts oj

the Pitch, Loudness

and Timbre of Musical Tones, N.Y.,

Telephone, 1935; ^- Kohler, "Tonpsychologie," Handhuch
Berlin, Alexander u. Marburg, 1923, 419—464.
Bell

20]

J. S.

Hurley,

"A Study

der Neurologie des Ohres,

of Pitch Tendencies in Certain Phases of Singing as Measured

by the Conn Chromatic Stroboscope," Thesis, Syracuse U., 1940. The picture with bowed
tones is somewhat different. Here professionals tend to flat with increases in bow pressure
but to sharp with each

bow velocity. See R. J. Harrington, "The Influence of
Bow on Violin Intonation: A Stroboscopic Study," Master's

rise in

Pressure and Velocity of the
Thesis, Syracuse U., 1952.
21]

O. Ortmann, The

H. C. Hart, M.
Tone,"

W.

Phjsical

Fuller,

Basis

and

J. Acoust. Soc. Amer., 6

W.

of Piano
S.

Touch

and Tone, N.Y., Dutton,

Lusby, "Precision Study of Piano

192^;

Touch and

(1934): 80—94.

Touch and Damper-Pedalling in the
(1929): 462-469; "A Discussion of
the Nature of Pianoforte Damper-Pedalling together with an Experimental Study of Some
22J

C. P. Heinlein, "The Functional Role of Finger

Appreciation of Pianoforte Music,"

J. Gen. Psjchol., 2

MELODY
Individual Differences in Pedal Performance," ].

Ten

"Pianoforte Damper-Pedalling under

Gen.

Psychol.,

2

(1929): 489-508;

Different Experimental Conditions," j. Gen.

Psychol, 3 (1930): 511-^:28.
23]

One

researcher believes he has demonstrated the existence of undertones, which he

conceives as the inverse of overtones.

C. Revesz's Introduction

to the

But so

far,

few others accept their

Norman, U. of Oklahoma

Psychology of Music,

reality.

See

Press, 1954,

pp. 13-1424]

For an excellent account of the timbre of band and orchestral instruments see

Chapter 17 of C, E. Seashore's Psychology of Music, N.Y., McGraw-Hill, 1938.
2 £]

The mounting

of the

mute on the bridge of

a violin

overtones and makes the "shade" of the tone "darker."

produced by holding the mouth "long," and a
D. Preston, "Pitch Variations

dampens certain of the higher
A "dark" vocal tone can be

"light" tone

in the Singing of Specific

Using Bright and Dark Tones," Master's Thesis, Syracuse U.,
26]

Variability, a general fact of

Many men
in life.

life, is

by

a "short"

Vowels on

mouth.

See

Specific Frequencies

1945^.

well illustrated in the area of auditory sensitivity.

deafen early while others keep their sensitivity relatively intact until quite late

But the average changes in hearing

ability are as described above.

C. C. Bunch, "Age Variations in Auditory Acuity," Arch. Otolaryngol., Chicago, 9
(1929): 62^-636; H. C. Montgomery, "Do Our Ears Grow Old?" Bell Lab. Rec, 10
27]

(1932): 311-313; N. H. Kelley,

Age and

Its

29 (1939),

somewhat

Effect

i'o6-5'i3.

28]

Montgomery

slighter than does

the frequencies

"A Study

in Presbycusis:

Auditory Loss with Increasing

upon the Perception of Music and Speech,"

below 1024

P. R. Farnsworth,

Arch. Otolaryngol., Chicago,

pictures the hearing losses of the aging as being

Bunch. Kelley presents them

as still slighter, particularly for

d.v.

"Notes on 'Coldness'

in Violin Playing," J. Psychol.,

33 (1952):

41-4?.
29]

M.

Metfessel, "Sonance as a

Form

of Tonal Fusion," Psjchol. Rev., 33 (1926): 45^9—

466. See also O. Ortmann's "The Psychology of Tone Quality," 1939

Int.

Congr. Musical.,

1944, PP- 227-232.
30]

Research

men

of the Seashore laboratories at the University of Iowa have

excellent studies of certain of these phenomena.

The

made

interested student can refer to

C. E. Seashore's Psychology oj Music, Chap. 9, or to the following monographs by Seashore's

student associates for detailed pictures of the portamento and of typical attacks and releases
of vocal tones: D. Lewis,

M. Cowan, and G.

Fairbanks, "Pitch Variations Arising from

Certain Types of Frequency Modulation," J. Acoust. Soc. Amer., 9 (1937): 79; R. E.
Miller, "The Pitch of the Attack in Singing," la. Stud. Mus., 4 (1936): 15^8-171; H. G.
Seashore,

A. Small,

"An Objective Analysis of Artistic Singing," la. Stud. Mus., 4 (1936): 12-157;
"An Objective Analysis of Artistic Violin Performance," la. Stud. Mus., 4 (1936):

172-231
31]
32]

W.
W.

T. Bartholomew, Acoustics of Music, N.Y., Prentice-Hall, 1942, p. 159.

G.

244-259-

Hill,

"Noise in Piano Tone, a Qualitative Element," Music. Quart., 26 (1940):

MELODY
33]

34]

H. A. Block, and

P. R. Farnsworth,

W.

C. Waterman, "Absolute Tempo," J. Gen.

10 (1934): 230-233.

Psychol.,

M. Lund, "An

Analysis of the 'True Beat' in Music," Doctoral Thesis, Stanford U.,

19393

5^]

For a definition of takt and true beat see Glossary.

36]

G. Brelet, Le Temps Musical, Paris, Press. U. de France, 1949.

37]

M.

T. Henderson, "Rhythmic Organization in Artistic Piano Performance," U. of

la. Stud. Psjchol.

psychologists

Mus.,

who

4 (1936): 281-305. The work of Henderson and the other Iowa

have studied piano performance was made possible by

For more

designed camera.

detail

Musical Pattern Score of the First
U. of
38]

la. Stud. Psychol.

It

its

J.

"A

of the Beethoven Sonata, Opus 2J, No. 2,"

A

Study of Musical Rhythms,"

J.

Gen. Psjchol.,

20

should be noted that the jazz musician syncopates not only with

Possibilite

as well.

See also P. Fraisse and

de Syncoper en Fonction du

Tempo

S.

Ehrlich,

d'une Cadence," Annie

5S (19?^): 61-65.

Psychol.,

39]

la

specially

4 (1936): 263-280.

Music,

rhythmic beats but with melodic contours

"Note sur

a

use see L. Skinner and C. E. Seashore,

Movement

H. E. Weaver, "Syncopation:

(1939): 409-429.

on

Eggen, "A Behavioristic Interpretation of Jazz," Psychol. Rev., 33 (1926):

B.

407-409.
40]

Kate Gordon,

Esthetics,

N.Y., Holt, 19 13.

41] J. L. Mursell, The Psychology of Music, N.Y.,

reproduced here with the permission of the
42]

R.

W.

Norton, 1937, p. 177. Mursell's table
Co.

Lundin, An Objective Psychology oj Music, N.Y., Ronald Press, 1953, p. 95.

43]

K. Dunlap, A Sjstem oJ Psychology

44]

See Kitten on

the Keys

or

/

Cant

,

N.Y., Scribner, 191

2,

pp. 309—313.

Give You Anything But Love, Babj.

45]

P. R. Farnsworth, "Concerning Cross-Rhythms," Sch. Mus., 33 (1933): 11-12.

46]

E. Jaques-Dalcroze, Rhjthm, Music and Education, N.Y.,

47]

This

is

is

W. W. Norton

not to imply that expectancy

fortunately, there

is

no brief

treatise

would do well

i.

the only basic principle of learning.

on learning which can be read with

psychologically unsophisticated musician.
learning principles

is

Putnam, 192

profit

Un-

by the

Those who wish more acquaintance with

to devote the time necessary to reading the learning

sections of one of the elementary textbooks in psychology.

83
6-2

CHAPTER FIVE

Language Aspects of Music

VVe

have already seen that sometimes to some people a melody

presents at least a small message.

said that

answer

still

is

a language in

question

we must

a

any

mean by language and then examine

this,

all

strict sense of the

agree on what

first

eerie feeling,

march time, or com-

But, granting

other impressions.

music

this

may "talk about" an

an oriental atmosphere,

a restlessness,

municate

It

we

can

it

be

word? To

are going to

the research material of the area.

Desire Jor Communication
Before the language process can be said to exist, there must be
persons
this

who

desire to

communicate

were the only earmark of

many composers, convinced
religious,

economic, or

their thoughts and emotions.

a language,

that the

political

music would

world needs

ideologies,

feel

If

qualify, for

their favored

that they

must

medium of their musical creations.
be found who believes that he, too, has

share their beliefs through the

An

occasional performer can

a share in the

communication process through

handling musical materials.

His thoughts, he

modify what the composer has to
that the confidence of

they can do

84

is

say.

It

unique way of

insists,

add to or

should be noted, however,
in

what they think

that they can successfully

employ musical

composers and performers

no proof

his

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
elements

linguistic signs, that they can tell the stories they are

as

so anxious to

tell.

different problem,

The matter of
which

somewhat

success constitutes a

will be discussed later in this chapter.

Grammar
A

second major earmark of language has to do with the formal

relations

among

music, with

harmonic

its

signs.

Every language has

signs.

its

grammar.

detailed rules governing the use of

But no grammar

is

its

fixed once and for

So does

melodic and

all.

The use of

"none are" would have been banned without question by any editor
a

few years ago.

Now

this

phrase occasionally gets by on the

rationale that usage seems to be giving
rules of music.

viewed

it

support. Thus

it is

with the

What was not allowed some time ago may now be
And some of the presently accepted usages

as acceptable.

may be frowned on in a later period. It is clear, then, that the rules
of grammar change. But, even so, there has been a grammar for
every school of musical composition.

Meaning
Few

but professional musicologists take

grammar of music.
historically

for

It is

much

a highly specialized area to

minded. More attention

is

communication. What motivates

likely to
a

interest in the

be studied by the

be given the desire

composer

to

create or a

professional performer to reproduce music arouses a deep-seated
curiosity
vity

on the part of many of us. Yet even motivation and

do not,

as a rule, excite as

much

interest as does the

musical meaning, the story that music
It

problem of

tells.

has been suggested that the existence of a desire for

communica-

tion does not of itself guarantee the presence of meaning.

monkey may

creati-

A

hungry

wail and screech in his attempts to communicate with

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
mate

his

in a nearby food-filled cage.

While the

monkey may

first

interest the second with his antics, his vocalizations will not tell her

to give

him

food.

So

far as she is

concerned

his vocal behaviors are

not meaningful although he

is

obviously striving to

The problem now before

us

is

communicating and

like the

hungry monkey,

must somehow

are deluding themselves into believing that others

understand them. The remainder of

with

so.

what extent musicians are

to see to

what degree they,

in

make them

chapter will be concerned

this

meaning aspect of music.

this

Alleged Key effects
In Grecian times, long before the day of free

tempered

scales,

a

melody was supposed

psychological character of the

was

Dorian mode with

in the

whole

tones, a half-tone, and

mode
its

in

to

which

modulation and

reflect

the unique

was written.

it

If it

arrangement of a half-tone, three

two whole tones

G, A, B,

(as in E, F,

C, D, E), dignity, manliness, courage, and self-dependence were the
qualities

thought to be expressed. But

if

the Lydian

used,

melodic message was considered one of softness and

the

Because he believed that hearing the Lydian

indulgence.

be harmful to man's character, Plato banned
Republic."^

Lydian

It

mode

There

is

is

is

that of

no doubt

our current major

that there

i.e.,

no person of the present day

what

in this area as in

effects

many

him "hear" what he should
It

is

is

his "Ideal

it is

who imagined

effects.

But

it is

also

apt to be so affected unless

Man is
not much

he "should" hear.^
another, and

scholars

"meaning,"

clear that

told

from

self-

could

scale.

were ancient

he

is

it

mode

of interest that the internal arrangement of the

they heard these psychological,

highly suggestible
of a trick to

make

hear.

of considerable interest that this notion concerning modal

effects persisted

86

mode was

even after the modes had been shaken

down

to

two

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
the present-day major and minor.

In fact,

the idea was further

extended to the several keys of the major mode. Thus, the key of
F major was said to be the key of the pastoral
Beethoven's Pastoral Symphony); keys with
nine sharps

made men

idyll (for

seven, eight, or

five, six,

think of heavenly matters

example

while the keys of

;

F minor and F sharp minor were the tragic ones.
It

not

is

to speculate

difficult

on how, before the advent of

temperament, one key was associated with Heaven, another with the
farm, and
a

still

another with tragedy. For,

melody whose do

whose do—re

ratio

is

is

9

as

to re as 8:9 will not
:

i

o

sound precisely

would have been

It

.

was shown in Chapter

course, there

would come

school of thought.

in

2,

one

simple matter to

a

attach psychological qualities to these differences.

they attached would have been pretty

like

But whatever

much pure whimsey.

time a codification of the rules of

Of
this

Pastoral music must be written in the key of

F major, the rules would read, and tragic music in F minor or F sharp

And

minor.
effects

"sensitive" people

whenever they knew

may

It

who

all

strike the reader as

would begin

that the key of F

odd

try to find the "proper"

to "hear" pastoral

major was employed.

that even today there are

composers

key for their melodic message.

with equal temperament, where

do: re has the

same

But

ratio in every key,

there is little possibility that key differences of a psychological character

can

exist.

It is

know whether

true that the listener with absolute pitch (p. gS)
a

melody

is

being played in E or F, and

in these traditional expectancies he
different.

The only other

may

if

he

is

may

versed

feel that the keys are vastly

possible differences arise

from the

fact that

equal temperament is almost never quite secured, and from differential
fingering.

Weak fingers may be

blacknote keys.

But such

slight stimulus differences

expected to yield psychological
It

seems

safe to

from modal)

given too great a task in certain multi-

effects

such

as

those described above.

conclude, therefore, that no key

effects of

can hardly be

(as

distinguished

any importance exist for the modern listener.

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
Major and Minor Modes

We have already seen that intervals,
minor.

as such, are

to intervals of the

major and minor chords, where they remained

as

But with chords and melodies there

is

curiosities of nomenclature.

more reason

to apply these terms.

There are three positions of the major chord
they are C, E, G; E, G, C; and G, C, E

—and

all



the listener a happy

structure

what

G; E

flat,

mood. The three

flat,

were thought automatically

in

minor chord

—with

to elicit

C

their different

somber moods. But

in the structures allegedly causes these opposing feelings has

come from Meyer, 3 who
G)

the major (C, E,

One

is

to i) and to the

well balanced with

There

is

in that the
3

much

(15^

to

3

flat,

symbol structure of

G, or A, C, E) with

"points" both to the

15-

or

its

5^

5^

its

have been uncovered during

this

But two

5^

or

3

is

not nearly so

statements would have us believe.

between major and minor chords

(2)
is

The

The

common as

of

ability

the older

structural difference

not the only variable which

suggests "majorness" or "minorness" to the listener.

examine further the findings which

sets

century which lead to a some-

that of the earlier theorists: (i)

major from minor

g—^—i 5 has

(i^ to

to i).

of worth in Meyer's suggestion.

what broader view than
to distinguish

of the few helpful suggestions

points out that in the just-intoned scale

1—^—3, while the minor (C, E

more ambiguity

facts

flat

key of

which aroused

positions of the

G, C; and G, C, E

never been satisfactorily stated.
has

in the

were thought by the

early theorists to have something in their structure

C, E

neither major nor

Centuries ago these modal labels were erroneously attached

cast

Let us, then,

doubt on the invariance of

the correspondence between chordal structure and modal affect.
Shortly before

World War

I,

Valentine, an English psychologist,

began to question the idea that

a particular

chordal or melody

structure automatically called forth a particular modal name.'^

His

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
he found, made many errors in their attempts to separate

subjects,

the major from the minor.

showed

more

About

fifteen years later, Heinlein

much more

Heinlein's

a similar observation. 5

made

extensive research

that even his musically trained subjects labeled as

minor

than a third of the major chords he presented to them, and

labeled major approximately

12

Furthermore, Heinlein located

a

key which sounded minor to

which were reacted

number of compositions

his subjects,

and Anitra

s

Fifth Sjmphorry,

in a

major

and others in minor

mode

Thus, a Sousa performance of

to as major.

Handel's Largo from Xerxes and the

from Dvorak's

per cent of the minor chords.

first

both

in

theme of the

largo

movement

major keys, seemed minor;

Dance from Grieg's Peer Gynt

A

Suite,

written in minor

mode, was described

as bright

own

was labeled melancholy by many and cheery by

Caprice Viennois

many

others.

and happy.

Kreisler rendition of his

Hevner, however, found that her musically trained

group and even her relatively untrained subjects usually did rather
well in separating the major from the minor melodies

(as distin-

guished from simple chords).^
It

has been noted that both the whole-tone chord,

the chief Siamese chord (which

and minor) tend to be

is

C,E,

mood

and

about halfway between our major

classified incorrectlv as

minor. 7 There seems

to be a fairly widespread misconception that whatever

must be minor. So much, then,

G sharp,

is

not major

for the invariance of the structure-

relationship.

Modern

research has brought to light three other variables which

can affect the success with which people distinguish major from

minor. These are loudness, pitch, and tempo. Thus, Heinlein noted
that the louder and/or higher-pitched chords

seemed more major

and the softer and/or lower-pitched ones minor. ^

wobbling the thirds and sevenths are
later

popular music to induce minor

American Negro music

is

tricks

employed

effects.

Flatting and

in the blues

Characteristic of

the alternation of ordinary major

and

much

thirds and

89

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
sevenths with "blues" (shghtly flatted) thirds and sevenths.

Other

techniques to create a sad atmosphere are found in some jazz compoBeale Street

sitions

Mamma,

for instance

ance of the leading tone. The tonic

is

from below,

a blue third, the sixth

—where there

approached
or,

more

at a

is

an avoid-

cadence through

rarely, the blues third

and the second.
That tempo

is

an important variable can easily be demonstrated

the reader will simply drag the timing of

some otherwise major-

sounding melody, or play rapidly some typical dirge.
tion of what had been

a happy-sounding tune will

be minor, and of the erstwhile

We

dir2;e,

if

His

classifica-

now more

likely

major. ^°

conclude that the problem of identifying the major chord or

major melody

far

is

more complicated than was once

thought.

It is

admitted that major chords and melodies are typically employed, in

Western culture
is

by no means

at least,

invariant.

on joyful occasions.
Moreover, the

But the relationship
nullified

by

color) notes and a chromatic scale.

It

effect can

be

deliberate changes in loudness, pitch, or tempo.

Color-Tone Linkage
Music has chromatic
also has timbre,

which

(i.e.,

in

Music has color, then,
literal sense in

connection

German

is

Klangfarbe, that

in a figurative sense.

But

which color and tone are joined?

is

Is

between any particular frequency,

is,

sound-color.

there a further,
there a necessary
interval,

chord,

timbre, key, or melodic sequence on the one hand and some given
color?

Many

scientist

musicians and poets of the past and even an occasional

have thought

in the "fact" that

so.

colors had seven names.

known
as

90

that there

well have

The

great

Newton saw such

both the diatonic scale and

is

named

no

his

own

a connection

classification of

But since the days of Helmholtz
finite

number of

colors.

eight or only four colors.

it

has been

Newton might

just

Nevertheless, the idea

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
that there

is

some inherent connection between color and tone

number

persisted and has led to the construction of a

which "play" colors
In the table

(e.g., the clavilux),

and sometimes tones

below are contrasted the key-color

eminent Russian composers."

It

has

of instruments
as well.

associations of

two

should be noted that they agree but

slightly.

RiMSKY-KoRSAKOV

Key

SCRIABIN

C
G

major
major

Brownish-gold, bright

Orange-rose

D
A

major

Yellow, sunny

Yellow, brilliant

major

Red

White

Rosy, clear

Green

E major
B major

Blue, sapphire, sparkling

Bluish-white

F sharp major

Greyish-green

D

flat

major

Dusky,

A

flat

major

Greyish-violet

Purple-violet

E

flat

major

Dark, gloomy, bluish-gray

Steel-color with a metallic

B

flat

major

Somber, dark blue shot with

Bluish-white

steel

Bright blue

warm

Violet

luster

Steel-color with a metallic
luster

F major
It is

the general lack of agreement

associations

the

two

of the

Red

Green

which

senses.

members

because of

attests the

There

is

some

among

the associations

much agreement

can be expected

similarity

family experiences.

That color-tone associations are very

many

those having tone-color

absence of inherent connection between

often

of a family, but this

common

among

investigations. In

common

has been

shown by

one of Omwake's surveys of college students,

60 per cent reported that they had color-tone associations.^^ Less than
I

per cent of the general population has color imagery of hallucinatory

intensity when stimulated
this tiny fraction of

by particular tonal

signals. ^3

The behavior of

our population has been termed "colored hearing,"

or chromesthesia, one of several possible types of synesthesia.
If

one can

would appear

trust the introspective reports of chromesthetics,

that

some

see a colored haze

it

whenever they are
91

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
when

Stimulated by a particular auditory stimulus. Thus,

the nine-

teenth-century musician Joseph Raft heard a cornet, he viewed the

world through

a greenish haze; the flute

produced

in

him an

ex-

perience of azure blueness, and the hautboy, yellowness. The trumpet
called

up

scarlet, the

French horn purple, and the flageolet grey.

Chromesthetic behavior tends to run in families, but
necessarily imply biological inheritance.

most frequently

Chromesthesia

with vivid imagery, and

and the presence of certain drugs,

fatigue, shock,

The reaction

in persons

is

one-way,

i.e.,

this

does not

is

induced

facilitated

is

by

e.g., mescaline. ^'^

with color never calling up

a tonal

In chromesthesia, higher tones are usually tied to

hallucination.

As

brighter colors. ^5

of the peculiarity's

colored hearing

as

claim not to have

a rule, chromesthetics cannot recall the date

first

Hence, they tend to regard

appearance.

inborn and universal and to look upon those
it

as insensitive individuals

who

who

are not realizing

their potentialities.

So

far as the

writer knows, no one has ever been able to induce a

chromesthesia under controlled scientific conditions. Kelly did what

he could along

this

line,

even going to the extent of giving his

subjects peyote and physical shocks in the

stimuli might help elicit the effect.
after

that these

added

2000 associations of tone and color (even 3000 for one subject)

became
In

hope

But no one of his subjects even

^^

a

chromesthetic.

an ingenious experiment by Howells,

either a high or a

low tone along with

a

subjects

green or

a

red

were given

light. ^7

Most

of the time the high tone and green light were given together and the

low tone and the
the pairings.

The

red.

But occasionally the experimenter reversed

subjects

were

told that their jobs

depended on

doing well and so were strongly motivated. They kept their eyes
closed until the tones

quickly

named them. Of considerable

that there

92

were heard. Then they examined the

was

theoretical interest

a gradual increase in the

number

lights
is

and

the fact

of naming errors

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
made

at the

times of the unusual,

But whether

green pairings.

high-tone/red and low-tone/

i.e.,

this rise in the error

curve indicates

the creation of a chromesthesia or of only a pseudochromesthesia

is

problematical.

The reason
persons and

few

for the presence of chromesthetic behavior in a

its

absence in most others

is still

not known.

^^

Anato-

mical, physiological, and psychological theories have been suggested,

Of course, there is the
answer may involve more than one

but no one theory seems entirely satisfactory.
possibility

theory.

the correct

that

For example,

whose brain

it is

conceivable that the chromesthetic

is

one

structures and/or physiological functions are such that

he makes tone-color conditioned responses more readily than do

most of

But he would also need to be a person for

us.

whom some

accident of environment had allowed the tonal and color stimuli to

occur together so that the conditioning could take place and
tonal configuration could

then,
fact

is

come

to

"mean"

one hypothesis. But whether

it is

a certain color.

a given

Here,

correct or incorrect, the

remains that no aspect of tone has, per

se, a universal color-

meaning.

''The Language of the Emotions"
Perhaps

it

time to consider a warning sounded on several

is

occasions by C. C. Pratt, ^9 an aesthetician

ambiguity in the idea that "music
Pratt notes that to

many

To

who

sees a fundamental

the language of the emotions."

mean that
commotion within

writers this expression appears to

the emotional character of music
the listener.

is

is

a subjective

others, including Pratt, the emotional character

objective property of the music
Pratt, in that the

itself.

The confusion

an

arises, thinks

same words have quite properly been employed to

describe the objective as well as the subjective events.

Doe and

is

a certain musical

Both John

composition may seem agitated, or calm,
93

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
But in Doe's case the

or passionate, or sentimental.

moods and emotions,
music

a

it is

of kinesthetic-organic forms, while with the

matter of tonal form.

The attempts of the

mena

formalists to keep these

two

sets of

pheno-

separate should not deny the possibility that on occasion

agitated music

may

one of

affair is

may

somewhat superior

take a

The

stimulate agitation in the listener.

attitude and imply that the sophisticate

moods and emotions

does not allow his

formalist

to intrude while

But the rank and

listening to the "best" in music.

file

he

is

of us are not

know of many occasions
improved our mood or when a sad-

functioning at such an "elevated" level and

when joyous-sounding music

has

seeming composition has elicited

A

a sad effect.

danger greater than that of denying the possible influence of

music on

mood and

musical character

lies

in the

assumption that

moods and emotions whose names
they share. Psychiatrists and clinical psychologists would no doubt
rejoice if all they needed to cure the depressed or the maniacal were
access to a variety of compositions whose "moods" had previously
tonal forms invariably arouse the

been carefully catalogued. Therapy under such circumstances could
be administered

in truly engineering fashion

one disturbance and Y for another.
invariant,

and the listener

is,

But music's

after all,

personal, as well as a cultural, history

any piece of music, to some degree

—composition
effects are

no automaton.

which makes

at least,

He

X

for

not so
has a

his reactions to

unique and therefore

difficult to forecast.

Years ago the great

Thomas Edison was

so concerned at

what he

considered the whimsicalness with which composers titled their

compositions that he hired a panel of experts to catalogue his
available recordings according to

labeled

1 1

2

"true

mood

"mood." Out of

music. "^'^

Fifteen

5^89

these, the panel

were guaranteed

to

stimulate and enrich imagination, 14 to bring peace of mind, 10 to

make one joyous, and
94

8 to elicit

moods

of wistfulness.

Ten were kept

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
in the list for jolly
1 1

for love,

I

o for

of tender memory,
spirit,

No

and

1

moods and good fellowship, 9 for more energy,
moods of dignity and grandeur, 2 for the mood
i

1

3

for the

mood

of devotion, 9 for stirring the

6 to "catch the childish fancy

and make

it

merry with glee."

doubt hearing the Edison recordings worked wonders on the

more

suggestible.

Yet Anna Case's singing of Home, Sweet Home,

supposed by Edison's panel to bring peace of mind, might well have

made the homesick person more distraught. And while some of the
more religious-minded no doubt found Schubert's Ave Maria conducive to the peace of mind Edison had guaranteed, it is doubtful if
all

so benefited.

In fact

one could be almost certain that the forced

hearing of this composition would

make

communist

the confirmed

^^

more irascible.
The mood elicited by

^
the music will depend not only on the tonal

configuration the listener hears but also on a variety of factors
external to the music

itself.

Among

the

more important

just preceding the listening period, the
if

there

music
musical

is

in

word-meanings of the

held

libretto

one, and the attitudes built up in the listener toward
general and toward the piece in question.

Although

compositions can quite properly be placed into

categories, they will not invariably arouse the

which they have been described.
that

of these

mood

variables are the listener's personality structure," the

music can be regarded

as a

We

moods

mood

in terms of

are thus forced to conclude

language of

moods and emotions

in

a very limited sense only.

Adjective Lists for Classifying Music
Research by Schoen, Gatewood, Mull, and others has demonstrated

beyond the

possibility of

doubt that synonymous words will

be employed to describe the character of most music whenever the
listeners are

drawn from roughly the same subculture. ^3 The degree
9^

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
of agreement

is little

affected by differences in listener intelligence,

tested musical aptitude, musical training, or age level
sixth grade). ^4

As might be suspected,

where the concern
passages

is

with program

which are characterized

and "calm."^^ "Defiance"

A more

more

is

in

listener

music. ^5

It is

relatively

bit of

in terms of a profile of the eight clusters

regards as appropriate.

The

list

It

than any

contains 67

been assumed to be almost

Hence, the characterization of any

adjectives, although the listener

poor for

The mood quale expressed by the

adjectives within any one cluster has

made

greater

than "rage" or "fear."

used in the past has been developed by Hevner.^7

identical.

is

terms of "yearning," "tenderness,"

easily identified

in eight clusters.

above the

agreement

systematically constructed adjective check

words arranged

(if

is

music

is

typically

and not of the 67

urged to check every word he

clusters are arranged like the dial of a

clock on the supposition that as one proceeds from any given cluster

around the

dial,

opposite cluster

the mood-similarity steadily decreases until the
is

reached; from there back to the starting cluster

the resemblance increases.

The

utility of the

by data from the

files

check

list

for the aesthetician can be illustrated

of the Stanford Laboratory. In one experiment,

200 college students were asked to
patterns.

It

number of brief tonal
show the responses to two

listen to a

will sufKice for our purposes to

of these musical passages. Only the adjectives checked by one-half or

more
First
all

of the listeners will be listed.

Movement

For the

of Franck's Symphony

in

D

first

but one of the adjectives were from cluster

dark, solemn, and mournful.
Clair de Lune,

different.

For the

2

measures of the

first

2

—heavy,

gloomy,

26 measures of Debussy's

on the other hand, the adjectives checked were quite

Here, two were from cluster

were from 4

i

Minor (allegro non troppo)



delicate,

graceful,

5^,

one was from

3,

and two

dreamy, soothing, and serene.

These checkings were quite reliably and consistently made.
to be expected, of course, that the responses

96

It

is

would have varied

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
Hevner Adjective Check

List

6
bright
cheerful

happy
joyous

merry

7

8

emphatic

5

agitated

delicate

dramatic

fanciful

exciting

graceful

exhilarated

humorous

impetuous

light

passionate

playful

restless

quaint

sensational

sprightly

soaring

whimsical

triumphant

4
calm

exalting

leisurely

majestic

lyrical

martial

quiet

ponderous

satisfying

robust

serene

vigorous

3

1

awe-inspiring

dreamy

dignified

longing

lofty

plaintive

sacred

pleading

serious

sentimental

sober

tender

solemn
spiritual

2

dark

soothing
tranquil

yearning
yielding

depressing
doleful
frustrated

gloomy
heavy

melancholy

mournful
pathetic
sad
tragic

97

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
somewhat had there been changes
were played.

passages

one which allows

All in

all,

in the

manner

the technique

a reasonably clear picture to

is

in

which the

a fairly sensitive

mood

be drawn of the

character of the musical fragments.

M ODIFIED

A DJECTIVE Check

List

A

B

C

D

E

cheerful

fanciful

delicate

dreamy

longing

light

graceful

leisurely

pathetic

happy

quaint

lyrical

sentimental

plaintive

joyous

whimsic al

serene

pleading

bright

soothing

yearning

merry

tender

playful

tranquil

sprightly

quiet

G

H

I

J

dark

sacred

dramatic

agitated

frustrated

depressing

spiritual

F

emphatic

exalting

doleful

majestic

exciting

gloomy

triumphant

exhilarated

melancholic

impetuous

mournful

vigorous

pathetic

sad
serious

sober

solemn
tragic

Hevner's check

list

has

been recently revised with

of the adjectives so that the

mood

new

a

rearrangement

more

clusters have considerably

consistency than the older clusters had,^^ In the course of the

research

it

was found that one of the

adjectives, "pathetic," fitted

almost equally well two of the revised clusters. The adjective
"frustrated" did not

fit

any of the clusters and so stood alone.

It

was

found that neither the original Hevner nor the revised clusters could be
placed in exact clock-face arrangement although the

98

new

clusters

came

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
No

closer than the old to satisfying such a scheme. ^9

goes on this much-used Hevner

as

time

added to and further

will be

list

doubt

improved

Which Give Meaning

Variables

Of

the variables

experiments in

modality

this area,

is

descending carries relatively

words, the listener

plays the

on numerous

probably second in import-

Harmony and rhythm

meaning to the

little

tempo

has carried

and whether the melody

importance,

less

to music,

who

Pitch seemingly ranks third.

ance. 3*^
far

which give meaning

According to Hevner,

largest role.

Music

to

are of

ascending or

is

listener.

In other

most likely to change the affective terms with

is

which he describes what he hears whenever

its

tempo

is

appreciably

slowed or hastened. Other alterations of the musical matrix change

what he

less strikingly

The
table,

relative

enough

is

saying to him.

is

no need to consider here the

by which she derived her weights; for our purposes

The following example

For music described

variables appear to

indicates

as dignified

it is

importance of the

say that they indicate the relative

to

variables.
:

music

reproduced below. There

statistics

table

feels the

importance of these variables can be seen in Hevner's

how

to interpret the

and solemn the most important

be firm rhythm, slow tempo and low pitch.

Major mode, ascending melody, and simple harmony are of

little

importance.

Gundlach has made

which give meaning
from asking
melodies. 3^

to

listeners

Out

a

somewhat
music. ^^

to

different analysis of the variables

He

factor analyzed data obtained

characterize a considerable

of this process emerged a factor which dealt with

A

second factor had

do with orchestral range and the use of certain

intervals, particu-

tempo, smoothness of rhythms, and loudness.
to

number of

larly firsts

and seconds.

A

third factor was related to the use of large

99
7-2

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
Gundlach found music

intervals.

carried

by woodwinds to be

characterized by terms such as "mournful," "awkward," and "uneasy;" by brasses as "triumphant," and "grotesque;" by the piano as
"delicate," "tranquil," "sentimental,"

and "brilliant;" by strings

as

"glad."

Relative Importance of Six Variables
Musical

Dignijied

Sad

Factor

Solemn

Heavy

Mode
Tempo

Major

Dreamy

Minor
Slow

Minor

20

Slow

14

Slow

12

Pitch

Low

10

Low

19

Rhythm
Harmony

Firm

18

Firm

3

Simple

3

7

Simple

Melody

Ascending

4

4

Complex



Major

16

Slow

High

6

High

8

Flowing

9

Flowing

9

4

Simple



3

20

10

Ascending

Graceful

Happj

Exciting

Vigorous

Factor

Sparkling

Bright

Elated

Majestic

Major

Pitch

High

Rhythm
Harmony

Flowing

Melody

Descending

21

6

Fast

16
8

Simple

12

Major

24

Fast

20

Low

Flowing

10

Firm

Simple

16

Complex



3

High Pitch

Pitch

Wide Range

'Narrow Range

3





21

Fast

6

High

6

Fast

9

Low

13

2

Firm

10

14

Descending

Pitch, Range,

Fast

12

Musical

Mode
Tempo

Low

Serene

Gentle

Sentimental

7

Complex

8

Descending

8

and Tempo

European

sentimental, whimsical, animated, glad

Indian

happy love, recitative

European

mournful, somber, tranquil, dignified, grotesque

Indian

general war, organization of war party

European

uneasy, animated, grotesque, brilliant, glad

Indian

general war, organization of war party

European

tranquil, dignified, delicate, mournful,

Indian

healing, scout, warpath

European

brilliant,

animated, uneasy, glad,

awkward, somber

whimsical,

flippant,

grotesque

Slow

Indian

general war, organization for war

European

dignified,

somber,

delicate, sentimental
Indi

1

OO

in battle, sad love

tranquil,

melancholy,

mournful,

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
Two

by Gundlach seem worthy of reproduction

They contrast the connotation of certain

here.
style

tables constructed

music with those

in the

music of

a

variables in European-

number

of American Indian

tribes.

Rhythms and Intervals
Characterization

of
Factors

Many Rough
Rhythms

Many Uneven
Rhythms

Appropriate Situations for
>

'

European Folk songs

Indian Songs

Musical Phrases

grotesque

after killing warrior

victory

uneasy

scout song, victory

war march

delicate

disappointment in love

death of lover

sentimental

parting

description

dignified

happy love

song

of,

or

to, love

exalted

somber

Few Uneven
Rhythms

flippant

after killing warrior

animated

recitatives

grotesque

victory

victory

brilliant

Many Smooth
Rhythms

Many

Ists

and 2nds

Many

3rds

brilliant

war medicine

lonesome or sad

animated

parting

gay or playful

flippant

death of lover

glad

healing,

happy love

uneasy

war medicine

mournful

death of lover

awkward

healing, recitatives

warpath

absence or parting

triumphant

absence of lover

victory

after killing

sentimental or serious
love

lonesome or sad

warrior

war organization

Many Large
Intervals

glad, exalted

disappointment in love

gay or playful

delicate

lonesome, scout

lonesome or sad
death of lover, war
dirge

From

the

Harvard Laboratory of Social

interesting study in

semantics. 33

Relations

comes an

Metaphorical terms employed to

describe voice qualities were taken from the writings of George

Bernard Shaw and three contemporary music

critics.

These were

offered to musically naive subjects for use in describing nine operatic

lOI

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
There appeared to be

voices.

among

three categories

potency, and

termed

—one having

a third

with

lump the

to

descriptive terms into

do with evaluation, another with

The baritone voice was often

activity.

"dull," "coarse," "closed," "dark," "heavy," "rough," "hard,"

and "thick;" the tenor
soprano

as

"bright," "thin," and "light;"

as "coarse," "soft," "light,"

Another attempted use of factor
ful

better than chance agreement

far

the judges and a tendency to

and the

and "thin."
analysis for studying the

meaning-

elements of music can be seen in the researches of Henkin.34 The

items to be intercorrelated were the preferences of college students

who had
classic,

listened to ten pieces representative of (a) Baroque, pre-

and

classic, (b)

romantic, and

(c)

modern

styles.

Two

inde-

pendent, meaningful factors emerged, which Henkin designated

melodic and

a

rhythmic factor. There was

third factor, orchestral color.

When Henkin

as a

also the possibility of a

was deciding on

his ten

compositions, he searched for recorded music written in a "purely

harmonic idiom" but was unable to

find a single

lack of records with strong emphasis
a

harmonic factor from appearing

The

example.

on harmony, he

It

was

feels, that

this

kept

in his final data.

data of a slightly later study led Henkin to believe that the

melodic and rhythmic factors are independent both mathematically
and psychologically. These factors appeared to be relevant variables
in

eliciting the galvanic

skin response.

Musical

style,

orchestration, timbre, and other compositional variables

have no significant relationship with

this physiological

dynamics,

seemed

to

measure of

affective response.

The Expression of Tensions

A number
ings of

of researchers have

music solely

in

felt that to

characterize the mean-

terms of the Hevner mood-adjectives

oversimplify. These theorists prefer to describe

io2

what music

is

to

signifies

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
in the

framework of

conflict, of the arousal,

While granting

resolution of tensions.

no concrete goals and no

nates

growth, change, and

that unfamiliar music desig-

universally agreed-upon

specific,

it

can carry a message of goal-seeking, goal-

blocking, and goal-finding.

We have already seen an illustration of the

imagery, they hold that

resolution of tensions in the discussion of the keynote

(p. 41).^^

But these theorists are more concerned with larger segments of
behavior and feel that music can depict, though but vaguely,

fairly

extensive experiential episodes.

Rogge

is

who

one

become impatient with the

has

analytic studies of music's
possibility that

she

a

To
terms of human

potentialities.

linguistic

music can communicate

developed

in

experimental design.

clever

earlier, highly

Bloch's

tensions,

Schelomo,

Ravel's Daphnis and Chloe, and Stravinsky's L'Histoire du Soldat

chosen for study. She
college students

whom

who

first

were

played these pieces to a group of eighteen

differed

the compositions

the

test

were

somewhat
unfamiliar.

in musical training

and for

After these students were

carefully queried about their reactions to the music, the interview

material was analyzed to extract similarities in content.

Out of

the

content similarities, two different paragraphs were constructed for
each of the three pieces. These six descriptions were arbitrarily

termed the "correct" descriptions.

Other students were then

called in to read these six paragraphs

and to decide which of Hevner's adjectives most adequately characterized the three

mood

When

patterns. 3^

two new paragraphs were composed
from the "correct" descriptions

as

agreement was reached,

for each piece. These differed
to narrative, i.e., in concrete

imagery, but could be characterized by the same Hevner adjectives.

Hence, they were termed "correct

in

mood

only." This

meant

that

Rogge now had four paragraphs for each piece, two "correct" ones
made up from interview material, and two "correct in mood only."
To make the procedure clearer the four for Schelomo are given below. 37
103

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
Correct

He wants to see
He wants to see

her open her eyes, to see her looking at him.

remembers

that she

too.

Remembers

the

years they had been apart, searching, following one another

from border to border. He wants to see her remembering
now the last few years they've been together. If he could
whisper just once more

memory
is

cold.

return.

how much he

him

of her love for

Her eyes glass
Once again he

is

over.
is

He

alone.

how

loves her,

deep inside him.

the

Her forehead

cries out, begging her to

He

can't believe

Then

it.

holding her hand, he begins to feel the strength of their love.

He knows

it

will be strong

enough

to help

him go on

alone.

Correct

As he watches the shore fade away he wants to

He

deck and sob.
exiled.

Why

Forever.

sit

on the

can never return to that land he loves.
hadn't they believed him

He

is

when he had

pleaded? Had there been one voice to defend his act? No, not
one.

Now,

rail.

Had they

the shoreline

as

lies

dim on the water he

been blinded to

all

his love

by their

grips the

own

hate?

condemn them? Can
now, regret his love for his country? No,
for so long as he knows that what he has done was right, will
benefit his people, then he can somehow endure the coming
Persecuted, suffering; in loneliness, can he

he fear

his future

years.
Correct in

The pigeon

flies

passed, and she

she

is

is

mood

only

back through the cold gray sky. The storm has

anxious to return to the safety of her nest.

When

over a village she hears a bell tolling. She wants to stop, to

descend and

rest,

but more than anything else she wants to reach

home. She passes over another town searching for the building
where she knows her nest lies strong and secure, and as she sees
104

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
the chimney, she circles and begins to settle. As she alights on the
ledge, and folds her wings, she sees that the storm has dislodged

her nest and the three eggs.
Correct in

Mood Only

As he approached her house he wondered
that night

How

on the

would she

Oh,

if

as

still

He

the stooped old

This wasn't her.

table

steps he

remembered. Twenty

when

He

she'd see him.

held his breath. The door opened.

woman he closed his eyes. It
He wanted to open his eyes,

he'd been mistaken.

The

Oh, he wished she might

?

had that strong, direct warmth.

probably shout his name
steps inside.

she had forgotten

she was before, as she was that night on

Going up the

she

if

straightened his well-clad shoulders.

react to his success

open the door and be
the lake.

He

lake.

below

years.

She would
heard foot-

As he saw

can't be true.
telling himself

But his eyes refused to open.

indicates

how

a third

group of some ninety

students reacted to the descriptions while listening to the three

compositions. 3^ They had been asked to select the three descriptive
paragraphs which best matched the three pieces of music.

If

chance

RoGGE Data
Description
1

Bloch

**16

2

Ravel
1

1

1

•4
*3

3

6

**28

S

2

*1

6

*11

7

2

*3
3

4

Stravinsky

8

1

9

**25

10

1

••49

IS

**48

3

11

1

1

••27

12

*11

4

2

90

90

90

Total

^05

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
alone had operated, seven and one-half votes w^ould have been cast

But that the voting was not

for each of the twelve descriptions.

chance

affair

which indicate the "correct"

starred positions,

show

single stars

a

suggested by the concentrations at the double-

is

descriptions

descriptions.

which were "correct

mood

in

The

only."

These descriptions received relatively few votes.

Rogge

Naturally, the

be

criticized.

study, like

One wonders,

all

had three pieces more alike in

guess

would be

that

It is

how the votes would have
mood been selected. A good

for instance,

fallen

compositions.

pioneering experiments, can

Rogge maximized her

by her choice of

effects

also quite possible that the votes for the "correct-

in-mood-only" paragraphs might have been more numerous had the

mood been

judged directly from the music instead of from reading

the "correct" descriptions.
In spite of certain inadequacies in the

probably

safe

to

Rogge experiments,

conclude that unknown compositions

it

is

may be

described not only in terms of the Hevner adjectives but sometimes
also in the language of goal-striving, goal-blocking,

of tensions. 39

Of

and the resolution

course, a descriptive narrative with

its

specific

imagery may not be the same for any two persons. But any narrative

with patterns of

stress parallel to the tension episodes of the

music

will be appropriate to describe the musical action.

Music as a Universal Language

We

have seen that the major chord communicates

its

"proper"

message only under rather limited circumstances, and that while our

major

scale

affect us in

may have symbolized decadence
such a fashion.

to Plato,

it

does not

Although the Siegfried motive may be

perceived by the devotee of the Rin^ cycle in the approved Wagnerian

manner,

it

will have quite different

meanings to those

been taught Wagnerian symbolism.
106

who

have not

Are there, then, no musical

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
configurations

which have

similar meanings to people of widely

dissimilar culture?

After considering the data of his tables, presented in the preceding

Gundlach concluded

section,

similarity in the

there

that

some

least

at

is

slight

meanings the cultured European and the American
But whether

Indian attach to the musical variables he had studied.

the similarities are extensive enough to raise them significantly from

chance

is

a question,

Morey played Schubert's Doppelgdnger and the
members of the Loma culture in Liberia

love duet from Tristan to

and found these "emotionally charged" compositions to have
emotional effect on the African

made

that had

natives. 4°

little

Yet the argument has been

he played other sorts of European music he might

have stirred their emotions.

When Dartmouth

asked laymen to draw forms

psychologists

suggested by hearing a series of twelve short,
selections, there

simple,

clarinet

was more than chance similarity among the forms

produced under the stimulation of any one of the

selections. '^^

Cowles likewise found some agreement among subjects who had been
asked to select a particular picture to match a given musical selec-

And,

tion. 4^

found

reversing

the

experimental procedure,

degree of correspondence among musical themes composed

a

under the stimulation of four designs,

e.g., a

saw-toothed form. 43

The commonality of response which appears
can be explained, in part at

would hardly seem reasonable

least,

on purely

to expect a

She would be likely to employ
else she

in these

experiments

practical grounds.

mother

It

to scream a lullaby

which she might belong.

to her baby, regardless of the culture to
soft tones,

monotony, and anything

and other mothers the world over had learned was sleep

inducing.

tempo could

scarcely

with Pratt's argument

(p. 93),

Similarly, a piece of extremely fast

mean "march"

to any

one might hazard
and an

Willmann

Italian

human.

a small

In line

wager

that

were

a Chinese, a

forced to listen to a series of tones

all

Loma Negro,
of the same

107

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
pitch and then told to

draw whatever seemed appropriate, they

would be more

draw horizontal

likely to

lines than vertical or

wavy

ones.

Psychoanalytic Symbolism
The

early psychoanalysts claimed to have discovered a

which

made

universal in

is

its

sweep but unknown

manifest through psychoanalysis.

universality

symbolism

to the individual until

The force of the claim

was somewhat tempered, to be

sure,

by the

for

inability of

among themselves on what symbolizes what. Yet
this lack of agreement did not deter the bolder of them from extending their dogmas to embrace most if not all human activities. For
the analysts to agree

illustrative purposes, only a

few of the claims pertaining to music

need be given here, since very
scientific

little

has been done by

way of

check. Thus, Montani holds that minor modes containing

the diminished third express feelings of the suffering, chastisement,

and pain which characterize reactions to the castration complex. 44

According to Mosonyi primitive and noninstrumental music
narcissism, and "good" harmonies "mass

symbols reflect obvious associations,

ecstasy. "45

e.g.,

On

signifies

occasion, the

rhythm and sexual

inter-

course, and in other instances, only an analyst will see a logical

connection.
Altshuler4^ and Tilly 47 have also suggested that music possesses
sexual symbolism.
are,

to

However, these

some degree

interest in such

at least,

According to them,

a

"manly" patient

has emotional difficulties should be approached with music

different

from what must be employed with

personality.

In other

and other music
io8

recognized even by laymen. Their

symbolism stems from the belief that music can be

employed therapeutically.

who

theorists feel that the symbols

is

words, some music

"feminine. "4^

is

a

man

of more "feminine"

"masculine" in character

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
A number

of psychologists have attempted to set up experiments

with the aim of indicating to what extent music can be described
terms of a mascuhne-feminine continuum. 49
studies

it

seems clear

that, ifforced to

it,

As

in

a result of these

laymen and musicians will

agree in selecting certain composers as the creators of predominantly

masculine music and others

as the originators

of rather feminine

compositions. Thus, Wagner, Beethoven, Shostakovich, Bach, and

Rimsky-Korsakov are regarded by many

most of which was masculine

as

having created music

in character;

Debussy, Brahms, and Schubert are

R.

Strauss,

Chopin,

more

classified as writers of

feminine music. By and large, the march, loud music, and the music
of the drums, bass viols, trombones, and trumpets are thought of
as the

more masculine;

rendered by the harp are

"decorative" music, soft music, and that
classified as feminine.

These findings should not be taken to mean that certain musical

phenomena

necessarily function as sexual symbols. 5°

data appear to
individuals are
as

show

is

in

the American subcultures polled,

sometimes willing to use "masculine" and "feminine"

category headings

there

that,

Rather, the

if

these

names are suggested to them and

considerable consistency in the

that

way they employ them. But

the making of such forced choices should not be interpreted as

proving that Beethoven's music

is

inherently and universally mascu-

line in any true sense of the term.

The

associated loud and low-pitched tones with

with male

soldiers, soft

music with women,

raters generally maintain that they

raters

presumably have

men, marching rhythms
etc.

When

queried, the

would have much preferred

to use

other categories than those of "masculinity" and "femininity. "5^

Summary
Music has

a

grammar,

authors of music, often

And its composers, the orators and
have much to say. To this extent, then,

a syntax.

109

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
music behaves

But does music convey detailed

language.

a

as

messages which are understood similarly by large groups of listeners ?
This

is

Key

extended argument.

a question that has aroused
effects, at least for the

objective fashion.

many persons

Nor

is

modern

listener,

exist in any

there a natural tone-color linkage although

associate a particular color with

and a few have associations of hallucinatory

major and minor

do not

some pitch or timbre
intensity.

However,
Chordal

effects are real, at least in certain cultures.

structure, loudness, pitch, and

tempo each

its

part in eliciting

the words "happy" and "sad." Moreover, there are a

number of other

and tension responses which music can convey to the

affective

Some music can even be described

relatively unsophisticated listener.
as

plays

masculine or feminine, although

this sort

of categorization seems

would excite any

to have little significance and certainly

none

but the Freudians.

clear that the "messages" of

music are

All told, then,

it is

in the affective rather than the cognitive realm.

stimulates no detailed imagery of a sort that

music

language,

a

that

then,

is

is

Music

widely shared. To

to distort out of

all

call

proportion the

meaning of the term.

The paragraph given below illustrates rather well the fallacy in the
thinking of those who would make music a language in the sense that
English or French
of the Boston

These notes, taken from a program

a language.

is

Symphony Orchestra, describe some of

the varied

reactions to Beethoven's Seventh Symphony.

Mark what commentators have found in the Seventh
symphony: One finds a new Pastoral symphony; another a new
Alberti

Eroica.

Germany

is

sure that

saw
it is

a knightly festival.

a political revolution.

Marx

describes a Southern race,

ancient
I

lo

is

a description of the joy of

delivered from the French yoke.

in

it

it

Moors of

Spain.

An

is

Nohl shakes

Dr. Iken of
his

Bremen

head and swears

inclined to think that the music

brave and warlike,
old edition of the

such

as

the

symphony gave

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
programme: "Arrival of the Villagers; Nuptial Benediction;
The Wedding Feast." Did not Schumann discover in the second
movement the marriage ceremony of a village couple ? D Ortigue
this

'

found that the Andante pictured

a procession in

an old cathedral

more

cheerful person,

or in the catacombs; while Duerenberg, a
prefers to call
Finale has

The

the love-dream of a sumptuous odalisque.

it

many meanings

North returning

:

a battle of giants or warriors of the

to their country after the fight;

a feast of

Bacchus or an orgy of the villagers after a wedding.

Ulibichev

goes so far

as to say that

Beethoven portrayed in

this Finale a

drunken revel to express the disgust excited in him by such
popular recreations. Even Wagner writes hysterically about

symphony

this

as

who knows

"Stroemkarl" of Sweden,

a friend of the

variations,

"the apotheosis of the dance," and he reminds

eleventh belongs to the Night spirit and his crew, and

one plays

eleven

and mortals should dance to only ten of them: the
if

any

and benches, cans and cups, the grand-

tables

it,

mother, the blind and lame, yea, the children in the cradle,
to dancing.

fall

"The

last

movement

of the Seventh symphony,"

says

Wagner,

We

are forced to conclude that, ordinarily, the images music

"is this

eleventh variation. "5^

arouses are specific to the experiences of the listener. 53

school of imagery

the motive-hunting Wagnerians),

(e.g.,

images will, quite naturally, be more alike.
is

several

have had similar experiences, are trained in the same

listeners

there

If

more commonality

that, as Pratt phrases

it,

On

their

the affective side

of response, at least partly from the fact

music sounds the way the emotions

feel.

Music can be used to indicate the build-up and release of tensions.
It

to

can, of course, be given almost any desired

stamp

however,

in the
is

proper associations.

Such

meaning

if

time

is

taken

specificity of designation,

not generally encouraged, for to most people the real

essence of music

lies in

the fact that

it

gives each person an oppor-

tunity to project his private experiences through his

own

personal

III

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
images or even to listen without trying to

elicit

images of any sort. 54

Everyone can appreciate the grammar, the melodic and harmonic rules
of the school to v^hich the music of his immediate interest belongs.

But he

not listening to a language in the fullest sense of that term.

is

Notes
i]

One

2]

For an exception to

3]

M.

F.

4]

C.

W.

reminded of the

is

legislative threats against

this

Valentine,

'n' roll

during the

s

of

Arithmetic, Boston, Ditson, 1929.

"The Aesthetic Appreciation of Musical
Brit. J. Psychol.,

Intervals

among School

6 (1913): 190-216.

C. P. Heinlein, "The Affective Characters of the Major and Minor

g]

summer

statement, see the next section of this chapter.

Meyer, The Musician

Children and Adults,"

rock

Modes

in Music,"

Comp. fsychoL, 8 (1928): 101-142.

J.

K. Hevner, "The Affective Character of Major and Minor

6]

Psychol.,

Chords," J. Gen.

Comp.

).

03-1

Psychol.,

C. P. Heinlein,

8]

1

Modes

in Music," Amer. J.

18.

R. Farnsworth, "The Discrimination of Major, Minor, and Certain Mistuned

P.

7]

4J (193^):

Psychol., 8

1

(1928): 377-379.

"The Affective Characters of the Major and Minor Modes
(1928): 101-142.

in Music,"

L. Kaiser, in "Contribution to the Psychologic

and

Linguistic Value of Melody," Acta Psychol., 9 (19^3): 288-293, argues that falling intervals

lead to sad affects while rising intervals the size of the fifth are exhilarative.
9]

Note that it is not the blues third per se which yields a minor effect but rather the
melody in which the third is embedded. Intervals as such have no modal charac-

over-all

teristics (p. 40).

10]

For detailed material on the

effect of

tempo

see

M. G.

Rigg's "Speed as a Determiner

of Musical Mood," J. Exp. Psychol., 2J (1940): J66-571. Rigg has verified Heinlein's
work on the effect of pitch in "The Effect of Register and Tonality upon Musical Mood,"
]. Musical., 2 (1940),

"sad" music in

No.
1

1]

Taken from

L.

468-48

more

See also K. B. Watson's extensive

Omwake,
1

.

P. A. Scholes, The Oxford Companion to Music,

Press, 1943.

I

12

Psychol. Monog., £4,

It

second edition, London,

Reproduced by permission of the publisher.

"Visual Responses to Auditory Stimuli," J. Appl. Psychol., 24 (1940):
is a slight tendency for persons who are

has also been demonstrated that there

interested in color than in

form to prefer tone

"Colour Form Attitudes, an Analogue from Music,"
13]

work on "happy" and

(1942).

2

Oxford U.
12]

49-61.

"The Nature and Measurement of Musical Meanings,"

to form.

See

W.

Austral. J. Psychol., 5

C. E. Seashore, "Color Music," Mus. Ed. J., 2S, No.

2

(1938): 26.

A. McElroy,

(1953): 10-16.

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
14]

J.

Delay,

et

al.,

"Les Synesthesies dans I'lntoxication Mescalinique," Encephale, 40

(19^1): i-io.
I

The

s]

fact that synesthesias are

modalities

is

emphasized by D.

I.

not entirely chance associations between two sense

Masson

in "Synesthesia

and Sound Spectra," Word, 8

(195-2): 39-41.
16]

E. L. Kelly,

"An Experimental Attempt

Technique of the Conditioned Response,"
17]

Biol.,

J. Gen. Psychol., 3

M.

J. Zigler,

Color Hearing,"

"Tone Shapes:

A

No.

3

Brit. J.

Novel Type of Synaesthesia,"

(1930): 277-287.
i

(1954): 289-300. Essentially the same position

Chapter £ of Aesthetics and Language,

21]

in the Light of a Case of

C. C.Pratt, The Meaning of Music, 'N.Y.,McGra.w-Hi\\, 193

Aesth., 12,

2o]

Chromesthesia by the

IJ (1934): 315—341.

5 (1933): 15^5—211; L. A. Riggs and T. Karwoski, "Synaesthesia,"

2S (1934): 29-41;

Psychol.,

in

Artificial

34 (1944), 87—103.

O. Ortmann, "Theories of Synesthesia

Human

19]

Produce

T. H. Howells, "The Experimental Development of Color-Tone Synesthesia," y. £xp.

Psychol.,

18]

to

J. Exp. Psychol.,

W.

;

is

"The Design of Music, "y.
taken by O. K.

W. V. Bingham, Mood Music, Orange, N.J., Thomas A. Edison, 192 i.
A more recent study along the Edison lines is that of A. Capurso, et al..

Your Emotions, N.Y., Liveright,

Bouwsma

Elton, ed., N.Y., Philosophical Lib., 1954.

1952, pp. 56-86.

Music and

Sixty-one pieces were found which

provided listener agreement (1075 nonmusical students were the listeners) of £o per cent
or more when the task was to sort the compositions into six categories.
22]

Thus

S.

and R. L. Fisher,

in

"The

Effects of Personal Security

on Reactions to Un-

familiar Music," J. Soc. Psjchol.,

those

who

34 (1951): 265-273, report that a large percentage of
react to "dramatic" music with unusually extreme favorableness or unfavorable-

ness

seem to possess marked personal

23]

M. Schoen and

E. L.

N.Y., Harcourt, Brace,
Psychol.,

24]

insecurity.

Gatewood, Chapter

7 in The Effects of Music,

1927; H. K. Mull, "A Study of

Humor

in

M. Schoen,

ed.,

Music," Amer. J.

62 (1949): 560-566.

R. E. Dreher, "The Relationship between Verbal Reports and Galvanic Skin Responses

to Music," Doctoral Thesis, Indiana U., 1947.
25]

P.

J.

Hampton, "The Emotional Element

in Music," J. Gen. Psychol.,

33 (194^):

237-250.
26]

I.

G. Campbell, "Basal Emotional Patterns Expressible in Music," Amer.

J. Psychol.,

SS (1942): 1-17; B. Shimp, "Reliability of Associations of Known and Unknown Melodic
Phrases with Words Denoting States of Feeling," y. Musical., 1, No. 4 (1940): 22-35'.
27]

K.

Hevner, "Expression in Music:

A

Discussion of Experimental Studies and

Theories," Psjchol. Rev., 4J (1935): 186-204; "Experimental Studies of the Elements of
Expression in Music," Amer. J. Psychol., 48 (1936): 246-268. (The Hevner Adjective

Check

List

is

reproduced by permission of the

Psychological Review

Psychological Association). A. L. Sopchak, "Retest Reliability of the
to Music," 7. Psychol., 44 (1957): 223-226.

and the American

Number

of Responses

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OF MUSIC
28]

P. R. Farnsworth,

"A Study of the Hevner Adjective

List," J. Aesth.,

13 (195^4):

97-103.
29]

The mood expressed by the

of clusters B and

"frustrated" describes a

expressed with
30]

adjectives of cluster

and resembles

I,

little

least the

mood which

mood

A

resembles most closely the

only cluster F reflects, and even here the affect

is

precision.

K. Hevner, "Studies in Expressiveness of Music," Mus. Teach. Nat.

pp. 199—217.

mood

of clusters E and F. The adjective

Assoc. Proc.

1938,

See also Hevner's "Experimental Studies of the Elements of Expression in

Music," Amer.J. Psjchol., 48 (1936): 246-268; "The Affective Value of Pitch and
in Music," Amer.J. Psjchol.,

49 (1937): 621—630; "Expression

Tempo

in Music," Psychol. Rev.,

42

(193J): 186-204.
31]

are

The

tables that follow,

reproduced by permission of The American Journal of Psychology,

from R. Gundlach, "Factors Determining the Characterization of Musical Phrases,"

Amer.J. Psjchol., 47 (1935^): 624-643; "A Quantitative Analysis of Indian Music," Amer.J.
Psychol., 44 (1932): 133-145-. See also S. DeGrazia's attempted analysis of Shostakovich's
His descriptive categories are intra-opus repetition, short and sym-

Seventh Sjmphonj.

metrical themes, figurative background, simplicity of fugal form, regular chord progressions,

rhythmic

background,

and

correlative

description

("Shostakovich's

Seventh

Sjmphonj: Reactivity-Speed and Adaptiveness in Musical Symbols," Psychiat., 6 (1943):
1

17— I 22).

32]

Factor analysis

is

a

method

for resolving a set of interrelated variables or tests into a

few "factors" which are regarded

as

being the fundamental variables underlying the

original

complex of

variables.

R.

W. Brown,

R. A. Leiter, and D. C. Hildum, "Metaphors from Music Criticism,"

33]

J. Abn. Soc. Psjchol., 54 (19^7): 347-3?2.

34]

R.

43
35^]

Henkin, "A Factorial Study of the Components of Music, "y.

I.

161-181

;

"A Reevaluation of

(195^7):

a Factorial

Psjchol., 39 (i9SS)'Study of the Components of Music, "y. Psjchol.,

301-306.

R. V. Fay points out that music tensions are produced by "dissonance and diminution

of dissonance, intensification of a rhythmic pattern, intensification of a note or chord,
sequential building of phrases or motives, change in dynamics, alternation of unfamiliar

material with familiar material, enlargement and elaboration of material already presented,

addition of

new harmonies

Development

or of melodic and rhythmic counterpoints" ("Tension and

as Principles in

Musical Composition," J.

36]

Actually, only a portion of the

37]

G. O. Rogge, "Music

as

Hevner

list

Alusicol.,

5 (1947): 1-12).

was presented.

Communication, with Special Reference to

Its

Role

as

Content," Doctoral Thesis, U. of California at Los Angeles, 19^2, pp. 66—67.
38]

G. O. Rogge, op.

39]

A. Pepinsky, "The Contribution of the Frequency Factor to the Psychological State

cit.,

p. 76.

of Tension," yWu5. Teach. Nat. Assoc. Proc. 1939, pp. 134-143.
40]

114

R. Morey, "Upset in Emotions," J. Soc. Psjchol., 12 (1940), 333-3^6.

LANGUAGE ASPECTS OFMUSIC
T. F. Karwoski, H.

41]
II.

The Role of Form

L.

Omwake,

S.

Odbert, and C. E. Osgood, "Studies in Synesthetic Thinking:

Responses to Music, "J. Gen. Psychol., 26 (1942): 199-222;
"Visual Responses to Auditory Stimuli," J. Appl. Psychol., 24 (1940): 468—
in Visual

481.
42]

T. Cowles, "Experimental Study of Pairing Certain Auditory and Visual Stimuli,"

J.

J. Exp. Psychol.,

43]

Monog., 57, No.

Psychol.

44]

18 (1935'): 461-469.

R. R. Willmann, "An Experimental Investigation of the Creative Process in Music,"
i

(1944).

A. Montani, "Psychoanalysis of Music," Psychoanal. Rev., 32 (194^): 22^—227.

45]

D. Mosonyi, "Die irrationalen Grundlagen der Musik," Imago, 21 (193^): 207-226.

46]

I.

M.

Altshuler,

"The Case of Horace

F.," Mus. Teach. Nat. Assoc. Proc. 1946, pp.

368-

381.
47]

M.

Tilly,

"The Psychoanalytic Approach

and Feminine Principles in

to the Masculine

Music," Amer. J. Psychiat., 103 (1947): 477-483.
48]

The ancient

theorists of China also

saw sex

qualities in music.

In the Lin

Lun

system of approximately 2700 B.C. in the reign of Emperor Huang-ti there were the lu

or masculine family of scale notes and the

Handschin
49]

lui

or feminine.

See also the theories of

(p. 5^2).

P. R. Farnsworth,

Musical Phenomena,"

and Femininity

J.

C. Trembley, and C. E. Dutton, "Masculinity and Femininity of

J. MusicoL,

9 (19J1): 2^7-262; C. H. Rittenhouse, "Masculinity

in Relation to Preferences in Music," Thesis, Stanford U., i9i^2; P. R.

Farnsworth, "The Musical Taste of an American Musical Elite," Hinrichsen Musical Year
Book,

^o]

7 (19^2):
I.

1 1

2-1 16.

A. Berg, in "Observations Concerning Obsessive Tunes in Normal Persons under

Clin. PsjchoL, 9 (1953): 300-302, has made a study of clients who were
plagued by obsessive songs and song lyrics. Interestingly enough, he found that their

Stress," J.

conflicts,

while severe, were largely nonsexual. This was true even though the lyrics of

these obsessive tunes

were

filled

with what the Freudians traditionally term sexual

symbols, e.g., bananas, coconuts, pistols, and the like.
£i]

A

cleverly written article

Symbols

on

art

— Freudian and Otherwise,"

and Freudianism

J. Aesth., 12,

on the same general topic can be found
£2]

No.

is
i

that

by R. Amheim, "Artistic

(195^3): 93-97.

Other

articles

in this issue of the Journal of Aesthetics.

A. R. Chandler, Beautj and Human Nature, N.Y., Appleton-Century, 1934, p. 213,

by permission of the publisher.
5^3]

Thus, persons

who

of Walt Disney and his

possess tonal-visual associations
staff

may have found

which

his Fantasia

English, ''Fantasia and the Psychology of Music," J. Aesth.,
54]

The

Evidence
age

(J.

calling
is

at

up of imagery

hand that

is

markedly from those

differ

extremely distasteful (H. B.

7 (1943): 27-31).

certainly not essential to the musical

this generalization also

H. Higginson, "The Associational

enjoyment of adults.

holds true for children 10 to 14 years of

Aspect

of Musical

Response

in

School

Children," J. Educ. Psychol., 2J (1936): ^72-^^80.)

115"
8-2

CHAPTER

SIX

The Nature of Musical Taste

l\ PERUSAL

of the current definitions of musical taste would lead

one to believe that there
authoritative.

no definition which can be taken

is

Even Runes's

Dictionary

of Philosophy^

statements which are quite different in meaning.
faculty of judging art

perience."

The second,

artist in his

works of

rules,

His

offers

two

first is

"the

through sensation and ex-

"the ensemble of preferences

shown by an

choice of elements from nature and tradition, for his

art,"

would appear

broadened to include

The

without

as

all

to be acceptable

if

men making judgments

the term "artist"

is

in the field of art.

term "preferences" should not be limited

to

momentary

pleasures, but should refer to an over-all, average set of emotionally

oriented attitudes, even including the strivings for appreciations not

who is endeavoring to achieve an
appreciation of Bach's music, but who so far does not enjoy contrapuntal music, has taste different from that of his friend who casts
as yet gained.

Bach aside

Thus, the person

as the

mere creator of

tinkly sounds.

Musical taste can be very roughly described
attitudinal set

Note:

The

as

the

over-all

one has toward the phenomena which collectively

material in this and the following chapter

and revisions) from Musical

Taste

:

Its

is

reprinted (with adaptations

Measurement and Cultural Nature by Paul Randolph

Farnsworth with the permission of the publishers, Stanford University Press. Copyright

1950 by the Board of Trustees of Leland Stanford Junior University.

116

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
comprise music. The communication expectancies one
attitudes built

up

one toward modal,

in

effects, all quite clearly

chapter

we

shall

examine

an effort to understand

Whimsej
The

or

form

finality,

a part of musical taste.

has,

the

key, and other
In the present

this larger attitudinal picture called taste in

at least a little of its nature.

Law?
may

fact that several of one's friends

enjoy only jazz and other

colleagues receive their greatest pleasure from the music of Bach

is

often brushed aside with an airy "What's one man's poison, signor,

is

another's meat or drink."

statement that taste

is

The assumption seems

But the diametrically opposed view
is

that musical taste obeys

According to

this

some absolute and unchanging

low order;

friends have taste of a

who

Bach; and a

more

still

—the

taste."

the geniusare on their

One's jazz-loving

a higher order of taste

loves the music of

set of laws.

critics,

—have discovered, or

what constitutes "good

to discovering,

by the man

—and the more common one

view, the musically elite

composers, and the musicologists

way

implicit in this

whimsical, and so without pattern of any sort.

is

possessed

Mendelssohn but not that of

higher status has been reached by those

attracted to the

who

are

works of Bach than to those of Mendelssohn.

Laws can be either absolute,

and unchanging, or

eternal,

relative,

culturally derived, and stable for only limited areas and periods of

time.

The professor of music who attempted

to

keep

his students

from being "polluted" by the music of Mahler and Bruckner
really

happened

—was

following

the

absolutist's

—which

conception

of

musical taste. His colleague was more of a relativist when he said to his
students

:

"I

much

prefer the music of the early nineteenth century to

that of later periods

.

Yet it might be well

with music of later periods, since
this

who

if

you would become familiar

can

tell

what

level of regard

newer music will have achieved by the days of your grandchildren

.

117

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
The

absolutist in musical theory holds that musical creations are

good because they follow metaphysical

or are in line with

rules,

For

certain of the principles of natural science.

we

metaphysical approach,

five,

elements

who

were

five,

on the i^rounds

—metal,

and political relationships

social

sample of the

can refer to the classical Chinese,

justified their pentatonic scale

and only

a

that there

wood, water,



fire,

and earth;

five

father and children, husband and

wife, brothers, friends, and ruler and subjects; and five political

terms



king, official, people, governmental affairs, and wealth.^

own

Closer to our

Western European sixteenth-

culture was the

century taboo against music ratios employing prime numbers not in
the series i-6.3

The reason

space has six directions

To make

offered, as

—above,

we saw

in

below, fore,

Chapter

2,

after, right,

was that
and

left.

use of a prime not in this series was thought to be contrary

to the Will of

God. Even

in

that "good" aesthetic taste

our

own time there are many who believe
who happen to

inherent in those of us

is

have been born under a propitious sign of the zodiac, but these
astrology-lovers are often a
influential

predetermining

in

answer, however,

For

it

little

it

vague
taste.

as to just

which

Whatever the

sign

is

most

astrological

does not check with experimentally tested

facts.

has been demonstrated that the distribution of the birthdays

of the musically great

is

not different from that of the

In their birthdays, the artistic

and the

inartistic

less gifted.

have been favored by

the same astrological signs.

Although

it is

would believe

in

quite probable that only the

more credulous today

such metaphysical principles

as

that a law of physics or of
is

a

still

held by

number

many

some other

theorists.

have already noted

(p.

21) that

of the important textbooks persist in describing the scale

of just intonation

as

the scale of nature and our contemporary

equally tempered scale as a

former can be derived,
118

We

the above, the idea

natural science underlies taste

compromise

in theory at least,

affair,

simply because the

from the overtone

series,

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
while the latter cannot be so rationalized. These overtone enthusiasts

seem to forget

tempered

scales,

other cultures have enjoyed equally

that

some of them even

removed than ours from

farther

the possibility of such rationalization, e.g., the Siamese and the

Although the physics of horns, reeds, and

Javanese.

strings did help

condition the early use of the octave, and perhaps a few of the other

with simple

intervals

ratios,

it

seems unjustifiable to

restrict

our

explanations of present-day taste to physical principles.

Attention has already been called to the interesting neurological
speculations of

they relate to

Meyer and

to the theories of Mursell, particularly as

finality effects (pp.

42,

_5"3).

So long

are not formulated to exclude habituation they

as these theories

would not appear un-

tenable, although in the present primitive state of neurology they

not seem currently to furnish
nature of

A

taste.

much

firmer statement can be

made about

in

there are experiments to

show

for the diversities of true beat.

his

muscles react.

(p.

for

5"),

that the latter cannot possibly account
It is

obvious that the capacities of the

biological organism set the general limits within

and

the relation

music and body pulse-rate

between the true beat

do

help to the understanding of the

But that they can

which man's

affect his taste

ears

without the

mediation of cultural forces has never been demonstrated. All the
facts so far

seem

accumulated by the musicologists and

social scientists

to point in a different direction, toward a cultural explanation

of taste.

The hypothesis
large measure,

that

contemporary

culturally derived,

taste in

music

is,

at least in

can be demonstrated through

the data of anthropology, history, and experimental psychology.
has been
tuning,

shown
fixed

diatonic scale

tonal
is

It

that the Occidental love for simple rhythms, careful
steps,

harmonies,

the

not shared the world over.

tonic

As

effect,

we saw

and the

earlier, the

African predilection for complicated rhythmic patterns was so far

out of line with the taste and perceptual

abilities

of many of the early

119

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
missionaries that they

commonly reported

the Africans to be arhyth-

The Chinese often appear obUvious

mical.

music which has

little

harmony

in the

to mistunings; they love

Western sense of the word.

Yet Orientals can learn to love Occidental music and, indeed, with

come

continued residence in America

to appreciate

Western musical

principles, and gradually to develop facility in the perception of small

auditory differences.

Conversely, the people of the Western world

more com-

often learn to love alien music forms, and to master
plicated rhythmic patterns.

tuning, the

American slowly

When

constantly subjected to poor

loses his pitch sensitivity

and

his

need

for pitch exactitude.
It is

well

known

that the people of each culture area are likely to

regard their art forms
neighbors.

as

God-given and superior to those of their

But the theorist

who would

accept the mythology of

racism and believe that the composers of his group alone have
discovered the "true" standards of musical taste will receive no

support from the social sciences.

Some form
that the

of the absolutist view

among the
whimsey view of

musicians.

matter of

It is

fact,

is

often accepted by professional

relatively untutored,
taste

is

more

on the other hand,

likely to

be found.

As

a

the evidence which can be gathered supports neither

of these conceptions. But before indicating what theoretical position
the facts force us into, let us see
taste

is

without pattern.

to be found in

We

why we cannot

shall first

accept the idea that

note the degree of agreement

judgments on relative eminence.

Eminence
The most eminent composer
even the

man whose works

are

is

between these three categories
ference

I20



is

a close one.

not necessarily the best known, or

most preferred. Yet the relationship

— eminence,

knowledge, and pre-

Moreover, while eminent composers are

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
assumed to be geniuses or

create that

which

at least near-geniuses,

Only

be called eminent.
is

a

few

live

not

up to their

all

geniuses can

potentialities,

honored by their fellows. Others have

or

traits

of

character or are subjected to situational pressures which preclude
great achievement;

moments"

or perhaps they are not born at the "right

development of their

in the

art.^

We

can readily accept

the traditional, dictionary view of eminence as "an elevated condition

among men;

a place

elevated status

is

or station above

men

in general," adding that the

the result of social evaluations which are relative to

particular times and places.

The patience of the
classroom and

listener, along

lecture hall,

with the conventions of the

force each lecturer on music history to

confine his talk to an hour or two.
restricted to

reasonable

number

what the author and

number of pages. These

of composers

who

editor, if

work

written

Similarly,

no one

else,

is

consider a

limitations function to reduce the

can receive

critical attention,

and to

decrease the extent to which those of lesser importance can be
treated.

with

as

By these means, eminence hierarchies are

many such

Each scholar has

lists

appearing

as

in effect created,

there are musicologists at work.

his individual biases,

produced out of

national,

school, and teacher loyalties, and occasional illogical reasoning, to

mention what are perhaps the most obvious
a

measured entity

is

to have

stability,

influences. If eminence as

each selection of the eminent

must be balanced by the choices of many others.
polling

of individual

decisions

furnishes

just

Fortunately, the

such

mechanism, and cancels out many of the eccentricities

EMINENCE RANKINGS. The
just a

a

balancing

in the choices.

unsounducss of the view that taste

is

matter of whimsey can be demonstrated in a variety of ways.

Our first evidence

is

that the

Society, one group of elite,
in their selections of

members of the American Musicoloaical
agree among themselves extremely well

eminent musicians.

In 19^1 these specialists

111

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
were asked

to consider a long

to check the

list

of composers born since

names of the ten whose contributions

to be of greatest importance. 7

the second into pile

I,

two

piles of the

same

2,

first

the third into pile

ballot going into
etc.

i,

Thus, with

two rankings of eminence could be con-

size,

number of checks each composer received

structed by tabulating the

from the

ballots in each pile.

The

below shows

table

870 and

As the ballot sheets were received,

they were put alternately into two piles, the
pile

i

music appeared

to

that the first seven

two subgroups of musicologists were

identical.

names chosen by the

The eighth and ninth

Of
composers who

of the one subgroup were the ninth and tenth of the other.
course,

the table had been lengthened to include

if

received relatively few votes, the two columns would not resemble

each other quite
relish roast beef

as

much

they do now.

as

For just

as

almost

all

of us

and apple pie but disagree more with regard to tripe

and cottage-cheese pie, so practically

all

musicologists treat with high

regard Stravinsky and Bartok, but do not agree quite

as

well on the

importance of composers of the order of Mason and Lockwood.

First Nine Eminence Choices of Musicologists

Polled in 195^1
Composers Born Since
'k

Order

'Note:

122

1870
Pile

1

2

1

Stravinsky

^Stravinsky

2

Bartok

(Bartok

3

Hindemith

4

Ravel

Ravel

S

Schonberg

Schonberg

6

Prokofiev

Prokofiev

7

Vaughan Williams

Vaughan Williams

8

Copland

Rachmaninov

9

Britten

Copland

11

first

Pile

Rachmaninov

The name appearing below

Hindemith

1

Britten

the line in any one of the columns appears

nine in one of the other columns.

among

the

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
The musicologists were

also asked in 195^1

to scrutinize

lists

of

composers whose birth dates were before 1870. They were to check
the

2

£ they

deemed most worthy

to be called to the attention of their

When

children and their lay contemporaries.

the two-pile scheme

was employed, the top names were found to be the following:

Eminence Choices of Musicologists
Polled in 195^1
Composers Born Before 1870

First Nine

Rank Order

Pile

Beethoven

2

Bach

Bach

3

4

Haydn
Brahms

Mozart

5

Mozart

Haydn

6

Debussy

Schubert

Handel

Debussy

r Schubert

Handel

9

[ Wagner

Wagner

A

were

the American Musicological Society

1938 and were asked

musicians of history

Letters

Brahms

8

The members of
consulted in

2

Beethoven

7

to music. ^

Pile

1

1

whom

again sent to the

more

were placed randomly

that time to
as

in

into

AMS members
19^1.9

two

sets.

The

list

first

the ten

having contributed most

composers were named in

total of 92

already noted, once

at

they regarded

were

in

this survey.

1944 and,

as

we

ballots received in

have

1944

Special attention was paid to

the ranks achieved by the 92 names mentioned in the original (1938)
survey.

The

correlation {rho)

the high value of •97.^"

between the two-pile orders came to

The same technique employed on the

195^1

of -96 for those born since 1870 and '98 for those

data yielded

r/70's

born before

this date.

It is

thus apparent that even at the lower levels

of eminence there was considerable agreement
between the two subo
groups of contemporary musicologists. It can safely be said, then,
that the

some

judgments of

this

group of the musically

elite are

following

sort of principle or lawful pattern.

123

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
The reader may

say,

however, that

one thing to prove that

it is

among musical specialists, and
quite another to demonstrate it among lay groups. To answ^er this
reasonable objection, the procedures which were employed with the
considerable agreement in taste exists

musicologists

were used

from psychology

in

194^ with

senior high-school students, and with
fifth-graders.

students taken

25-0 college

with

classes at Stanford University,

Here, again, each group was

ballots cast for the 9

split into

composers voted

2

nearby

100 sixth-graders and 100

scheme was followed of correlating two eminence
from the

105^

as

two and the

lists

compiled

most eminent

in

1938. While no correlations quite as high as the -98 of the musicologists

appeared, the college value was found to be -9^, the high-

school -93, the sixth-grade -93, and the fifth-grade '87.^^

assumed that these groups are typical of
grade-school groups, even in California.
that since

other

all

the above groups

fairly similar lay

all

It

is

not

college, senior-high, or

But

it is

logical to believe

showed marked communality of taste,

groups would also show

fair

agreement among

themselves.

For

illustrative purposes,

number

the nine names receiving the highest

of college votes from a 195^3-^4 balloting are

shown below. ^^

First Nine Eminence Choices of 120 College

Students Polled

in 195^3-5^4

Composers Born Before
Rank Order

Pile

1870

Pile 2

1

1

Beethoven

Beethoven

2

Bach

Bach

J

3

(

Chopin

Chopin

4

i

Mozart

Mozart

Wagner

5

^

Tchaikovsky

6

r

Brahms

Brahms

7

[

Liszt

Tchaikovsky

8

r

Wagner

Handel

9

I

Schubert

Liszt

10

124

Handel

10-S

Schubert

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
The

grammar-school students agree so well

composers can only be interpreted
is

and even high- and

fact that musically elite, college students,

patterned and

is

as

eminent

this facet of taste

not a matter of whimsey.

Eminence hierarchies of great

stability also

similar culture are queried about the

The next

classics."

in their selections of

showing that

table

appear

when people

of

composers of the "popular

shows the top nine names given by two

college groups of approximately 40 each, questioned in 195^4.

subject had been given a

list

Each

on which were the names of 108

composers and had been told to rank the ten most eminent from

i

to 10.13

First Nine Choices of 80 College Students,

Polled in Summer of 19 ^4
Composers of "Popular Classics"
Rank Order

Pile

1

George Gershwin

George Gershwin

2

Cole Porter

Irving Berlin

3

Jerome Kern

Jerome Kern

4

Irving Berlin

Richard Rodgers

5

r

Richard Rodgers

Cole Porter

6

[

Stephen Foster

Stephen Foster

7

Hoagy Carmichael

8

S.

Romberg

9

J.

Strauss

10-5

Sir

CHANGES
folkways

Pile 2

1

it

IN

1^

Arthur Sullivan

Romberg

S.
(

10

EMINENCE RANKINGS.

Hoagy Carmichael
Sir

J.

Arthur Sullivan

Strauss

If tastc

bchavesas do Other

should be possible to perceive changes over the years.

That such changes do occur

is

shown by the

fact that

although the

eminence ranks yielded by the votes of the American Musicological
Society's

members

in 195^1 correlated '95^

with the ranks obtained

in

1944, they correlated only 'S^ with the 92 ranks derived from the
polling of

1938.

(The comparisons concerned the

1938 poll's

92 eminent names.)

12^

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE

Eminence Ranks of the Musicologists
tk

193s

Order

1951

1944

Bach

1

Bach

Beethoven

2

Beethoven

Beethoven

Bach

3

Wagner

Mozart

Brahms

4

Mozart

5

Palestrina

Wagner
Haydn

Mozart

6
7

Haydn
Brahms

8

Monteverdi

Schubert

Handel

9

Debussy

Handel

Wagner

10

Schubert

10

Debussy

10

Palestrina

11

Handel

25

Monteverdi

IS

Monteverdi

r

(^

Haydn

Brahms

(

Palestrina

Stanford college students similarly polled

(^

Schubert

Debussy

showed the following

agreements: 1953 and 1945^, a rho of -88; 195^3 and 1938,

a rho

of '81.

Eminence Ranks of College Students
Rank Order

1938

i945

.

i953

1

Beethoven

Beethoven

Beethoven

2

Bach

Wagner

Bach

3

Wagner

Mozart

Chopin

4

Mozart

Bach

Mozart

Wagner

S

Brahms

Chopin

6

Tchaikovsky

Tchaikovsky

7

Schubert

Brahms

8

Chopin

Mendelssohn

Liszt

9

Mendelssohn

Schubert

Schubert

11

10

Liszt

reverence for the
remark that "the older

a

past.

Liszt

One

{
-!

1

17

Tchaikovsky

Brahms

Mendelssohn

occasionally hears a cynic

composition the more highly will

it

be

regarded," or, "Music of the quality of Mozart's compositions but

written in the twentieth century will not receive wholehearted
acceptance."

Unfortunately, the complete facts which could prove

or contradict these statements are not at hand. Yet data are available

126

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
which bear
to the

at least indirectly

on the problem. They

refer,

however,

eminence of composers rather than to the acceptability of the

compositions.

one pertinent study, the time which had elapsed since the birth

In

of each of the 92 composers mentioned
polling of the
lated. ^4

most eminent

as

American Musicological Society was

Xhe rank order which

in the

1938

carefully tabu-

these time periods yielded was then

correlated successively with each of

more

than 20 eminence ranks

obtained through a variety of methods. The coefficients of correla-

were found

tion

to range



coefficient falling at

zero.



from +*2i
a value

i 5",

— '36,

to

with the median

not significantly different from

Had time from death been used

in the tabulations instead of

time since birth, the values would have been almost precisely the
same, since the two time series correlate '99 one with the other.

The

picture,

over-all

between the

relative

then,

disclosed

no

eminence of the men

significant

relationship

in this highly selected list

and elapsed time since birth or death.

eminence was

In a second study, the relation of the year of birth to

observed by plotting the birth years of the 92 selected composers,

and then finding the central tendency. The median birth year was

found to
a larger

fall

in the

number

determine the
For

decade of the i72o's.^5 Next, the birth years of

of less rigidly selected musicians were analyzed to

effects of

this part of the

broadening the concept of "high eminence."

research the original

was expanded to include
by current musicologists

1 1

8

list

other composers mentioned

in their articles

birth year of this less highly selected group

the i8oo's.
still

of 92 composer names

Finally, to study the effect of

more, the birth years of

all

as

eminent

and books. The median
fell in

the

first

decade of

broadening the selection

the musicians mentioned in

current encyclopedias of music, the Oxford Companion
the International Cyclopedia of Music and Musicians,

to

two

Music and

were examined.

For these two large groups the median birth years were in the

late

127

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
i8 2o's and the early 185^0's, respectively.

mode was found

examined, the

to

In each of the four lists

several decades after the

fall

median.

Another view of the working of the same principles can be obtained by studying the birth dates of the hundred or so

who ranked

highest in the American Musicological Society's eminence
5"

persons

who

was 1770; for the top

1

75^6.

1

1

1809; top 2^, 181

15^,

3;

,

top 100, 1820. The

comparable medians compiled from the data of the
astonishingly similar: top g,

17^6; top 11,

top 2^,

I

The

the

hundred ranks because of the poorer

first

8 10;

top 100,

I

818.

The

944 survey were found to
For the top 1 the median figure

achieved top status in the

have a median birth year of

lists.

analyses

i95"i

survey were

1770; top

1797;

15-,

were not carried beyond
reliability of the

lower

rank positions.

From

the data of these researches

we

can conclude that, in con-

sidering musicians of great eminence, there

is little

or no tendency

bestow most honor on those who have been longest dead.

to

clear,

however, that the higher the selection of those

eminent, the greater the chance that they will not be of our
In fact, the data

show

that

it is

far

we
own

call

day.

easier for a composer of a century

or so ago to achieve a placement in a music encyclopedia than
for

It is

one born toward the end of the

last

century or

later.

it is

The com-

poser of recent years has not had the requisite time to become
familiar to his listeners, to build

of
far

many

centuries ago

removed,

is

up

a school of supporters.

also at a disadvantage, for his

stylistically,

His peer

works are too

from the contemporary scene. The

latter'

become

progressively slighter as his

birth date recedes farther and farther

from the 1700's. The most

chances of being rediscovered

honored, then, must be the between-groups, the composers of the
eighteenth and nineteenth centuries.

128

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
Enjoyment
For those
is

who

beheve that the measurement of eminence

refuse to

an adequate measurement of contemporary taste, there

is

pertinent

information to be considered which concerns the closely related
variables of

enjoyment and knowledge.

ratings are positively correlated.

Enjoyment and eminence

But their degree of relationship

with the musical sophistication of the subjects.

varies

Data on enjoyment have been obtained from students of the San
Francisco City College. ^^ These subjects were asked to check a

of

22

5"

names and

to indicate the ten

most enjoyed. The
was

-66,

lower

showing

rho

fair

list

composers whose music they

between the rank orders of two subgroups

agreement.

It

should be noted that Bach

in this sort of ranking than in the

eminence

falls

listings.

First Nine Preferences of 126 Junior-College

Students Polled in
1^

Order

Pile

195^4
Vile 2

I

1

Chopin

Chopin

2

Beethoven

Beethoven

3

Tchaikovsky

4

Brahms

^Tchaikovsky

5

Mozart

1

6

Rimsky-Korsakov

7

Sousa

8

-

Schubert

Brahms
R. Strauss

Debussy

fSousa

[Bach

9

Mozart

|Foster

10-5

Foster

11-5

Debussy

12-5

Schubert

11-5

Bach

15-5

R. Strauss

15-5

Rimsky-Korsakov

The well-known West Coast

critic

Alfred Frankenstein has also

found Bach relatively lower in the enjoyment
interviewing
States.

many

lists

he assembled by

college students in different areas of the United

^7

129

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
As another example of how similar groups

will

show

similar

members of
Symphony Orchestra in
former symphony player. ^^ The

preferences, let us examine the ballots collected from

the Philadelphia Orchestra and the Boston
the early 1930's by Folgmann, a

members

of these orchestras were asked to exclude from their

preference ratings of composers any biases based on their knowledge

As can be seen from the

of the relative greatness of the composers.

accompanying
order

is

table, the first nine

somewhat

names are

dissimilar for the

identical, although the

members

of these two great

orchestras.

Preferences of Symphony Players
Rank Order

Boston

Philadelphia

1

Beethoven

Beethoven

2

Wagner

Brahms

3

Bach

Bach

4

Mozart

Mozart

5

Brahms

Wagner

6

Debussy

Schubert

7

Schubert

Haydn

8

Haydn
Schumann

Schumann

9

The enjoyment of

sections

Debussy

of compositions, ^9 of the simpler

phrases, and even of chords and intervals,
similar groups.

is

also rather consistent for

Let us compare the order of preference established

for college students at the University of Minnesota in the years

before 1929^*' with the order found at Stanford University in 1933.^^

While there
is

essential

regarded

Some

is

some

slight variation in the

agreement

as neutral,

as to

which

order of preference, there

intervals are preferred,

which are

and which are disliked.

years ago, while

making

studies of the Measure of Consonance

from Seashore's well-known music-test battery and of Kwalwasser's
Melodic and Harmonic Sensitivity tests,

"errors" made by two

130

an analysis was run of the

large groups of subjects. ^^

In these experi-

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
ments, the Measure of Consonance was treated
just as

as a

tonal-preference test,

were the two Kwalwasser records. The "errors"

(i.e.,

the

disagreements with the "correct" choices) of two groups of loo
subjects each correlated in this order: Consonance, -96; Melodic
Sensitivity, '98;

Harmonic

the Kwalwasser- Dykema

Sensitivity, '99.

Test

A

later, similar study of

of Melodic Taste yielded coefficients of

correlation of -78 for fifth-graders, '92 for eighth-graders, and -91
for college students. ^3

Dykema

Test

The corresponding

of Tonal Movement

Here again we

were

values for the Kwalwasser-

•91, •91, and -98, respectively.

see almost perfect agreements, this time in the liking

for intervals, for simple phrases, and for certain resolutions as well.

Preferences for Intervals
Rank Order

Minnesota

Stanford

Major Third

Major Third

Minor Third
Minor Sixth

4

Minor Third
Minor Sixth
Major Sixth

5

Fourth

Octave

6

Tritone

Major Sixth

7

Fifth

Tritone

8

Octave

Fifth

9

Major Second

Major Second

10

Minor Seventh

11

Major Seventh

12

Minor Second

Minor Seventh
Major Seventh
Minor Second

1

2
3

Fourth

Knowledge of Composers
Like enjoyment, knowledge of composers seems to be a factor in

known composers, however, do not always
appear at the top of the eminence listings, and when knowledge and
eminence ranks are correlated, the value may run as low as •65-. The
musical

taste.

The

best

next table shows the top nine composers

as

college ratings of professed knowledge.

considered, the rank orders of the

two

they appeared on certain

When

2

1

2

names were

piles correlated at 'JS-^'^

131
9-2

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
Familiar Names, Judged by 126 Junior-College
Students Polled in 195^4
mk

Order

Pile

1

Chopin

Pile 2

1

Chopin
Beethoven

2

Beethoven

3

Mozart

4

Schubert

Schubert

5

Brahms

Tchaikovsky

6
7

Tchaikovsky

8

Schumann

9

Bach

j

[

Brahms

R. Strauss

[

J

Schumann
Bach

(^Mendelssohn

Mendelssohn

10

Mozart

10

R. Strauss

Programs
By

this

time the reader may be saying to himself: "So

behavior under observation has been purely verbal.
taste

from the standpoint of action?

orchestras, for example, ignore

many

Why

far the

not discuss

Does any one of our great

of the

more eminent composers

and concentrate consistently on playing the music of certain others

Do

the programs of the several great

world tend

To
made

to

?

symphony orchestras of the

resemble one another?"

give a partial answer to the

first

of these queries a study was

of the frequencies with which the compositions of each of the

92 world-famous composers have been listed in the programs of the

Boston Symphony Orchestra. The tabulations, made by decades,

show rank-order

correlations from one decade to the next averaaino

in the nineties ('87 to '98).

In the table

below are given the names

of the nine composers whose music was played most frequently in

each of two decades.

names are
identical.

132

And

again

we

practicallv the same.

can

make

In fact,

the statement that the

the

first

ten names are

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE

Program Choices of the Boston
Symphony Orchestra
ink

1925-1935

Order

1935-1945

1

Beethoven

2

Brahms

3

Wagner

j

Mozart

4

Bach

[

Sibelius

5

R. Strauss

R. Strauss

6

Ravel

Ravel

7

Mozart

8

Sibelius

1"

9

Stravinsky

(

Beethoven

Brahms

Wagner
Stravinsky

«

Tchaikovsky

10

Tchaikovsky

Bach

10

Furthermore, supplementary information shows that the programs
of the great orchestras

do resemble each other

Mueller and Hevner have constructed
the programs played

American symphony

during

a "popularity

1936-41

orchestras. ^5

in

large

part.

pyramid" from

by seven of our leading

Note

that nine of the top ten

names of their pyramiid appear among the ten played most often by
the Boston

Symphony Orchestra during the decade

starting in 1935".

Seven American Symphony Orchestras, 1936-1941
Rank Order
1

2

Brahms

3

Mozart

4

Wagner

5

Tchaikovsky

6

7

it

Sibelius

JR. Strauss

8

(Bach

9

|Ravel

10

Thus

Composers

Beethoven

(Schumann

appears that this operational or action aspect of taste show^s

consistencies just as does the

more

verbal side,

which

is

studied

through balloting.
133

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
To
music

learn
also

whether or not the broadcast programs of recorded

show

consistency, an analysis was

made by

the author of

the Pacific Gas and Electric Company's programs, which, for a

period of years, were broadcast nightly over

KYA

from 8.00

until

10.00 P.M. The years 1941, 1942, and 1943 were chosen for study. ^^

Ranks for each of these years were constructed by counting the

number of times

the works of the great composers were broadcast.

Great consistency was found, shown by the fact that the average of
the intercorrelations of the three ranks was "9^.
radio hours of this type
is

show

similar

If,

then, other

program hierarchies, and there

no reason to suspect that they do not,

it

can be said that the choice

of recorded music for broadcasting purposes also follows

some

sort of

lawful pattern.

Pacific Gas and Electric
Rank Order

Company Broadcasts
1942

1941

1943

1

Beethoven

Beethoven

2

Tchaikovsky

Tchaikovsky

Brahms

3

Brahms

Bach

Mozart

4

Mozart

Mozart

Bach

5

Sibelius

Brahms

Tchaikovsky

6

Bach

Sibelius

Wagner

7

Wagner

Schubert

8

Wagner
Schumann

Debussy

Sibelius

9

Schubert

R. Strauss

Debussy

Beethoven

11

R. Strauss

10

Schubert

10

Schumann

37-5

Debussy

11

Schumann

11

R. Strauss

CHANGES
clearly in

IN

PROGRAMS.

program trend

Fluctuations in musical taste Can be Seen

lines.

If taste

were

a

matter of the slow

discovery of absolute values, one might reasonably expect that the
great orchestras

works of

all

would show

would gradually discard from

their

programs the

but the "best" composers. This progressive elimination
itself in

upward swings on the curves of the chosen few.

However, when Mueller and Hevner examined the 1876— 194
134

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
programs of several of the great American symphony orchestras,
curve fluctuations and plateaus rather than persistent climbs or

seemed

to be the rule. ^7

flat,

who

Beethoven,

started at the top in favor,

dropped speedily

position, though his curve

and

now

had

a dip in the late i88o's,

has leveled

vs^as

falls

show^n to be

But the curves for Mozart and Bach were

slowly climbing in favor.
quite

true that Brahms

It is

Wagner

off.

at first,

is still

then

in this

less rapidly

started second in popularity, then

climbed back into second place with

peak about 19 lo, and since has

fallen

appeared to resemble Wagner's, but

a

markedly. Tchaikovsky's curve
at a

lower

level of popularity.

These researchers have conducted many other interesting studies on
taste,

among them an

analysis of the relative popularity of Beethoven's

nine symphonies as program material.

For

illustrative purposes, it

will probably suffice to describe the popularity curves of the Fifth

and Ninth. The former had

dropped
until

a bit in the late

World War

I,

i

fairly

high status in the earliest years,

88o's, climbed in popularity rather rapidly

and has since

fallen slowly

preferred to any of the other symphonies.

what below the

now

is

Fifth

and has rather steadily

it is still

much

Ninth started some-

fallen in

rank until

it

lowest in favor.

In the course of a later study,

show

though

The

Mueller assembled additional data to

the cultural nature of taste. ^^

strike different world areas at

Just as social attitudes tend to

somewhat different times,

so,

he finds, do

trends in musical taste. Thus, while Beethoven's curve of popularity

New York
orchestras, any given position on the curve is reached by the New York
is

much

the same whether plotted for the

orchestras approximately five years after the

London or the

London body. For Wagner

the lag appears to be in the same direction, but

We

is

fifteen years in length.

have seen that the relative frequencies with which the works

of eminent composers appeared on the programs of the Boston

Symphony Orchestra

for the decade beginning in

those for the next decade.

The

192^ are similar to

data of the following table indicate

13^

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
that

tend to be greatest between contiguous

the resemblances

decades.

The program

present.

The

slow and irregular, but definitely

shifts are

correlation values

shown below were obtained from

comparisons of the relative frequency of appearance of the works of
92 composers on the programs of

this orchestra.

Decade Intercorrelations for the Boston
Symphony Orchestra
Period

Decade

Starting in 1895

Decade

Starting in 1905

Decade

Starting in 1915

Decade

Starting in 1925

1905

1915

1925

1935

•87

•82

•81

•81

•92

•83

•83

•98

•90
•90

...

Space Allocations
Another operational or action aspect of
amounts of space allotted to composers
general and music encyclopedias.

As

concerns the relative

taste

in histories of
it

is

music and in

obviously out of the

question to permit the insertion in these books of articles of con-

on

siderable length

of the world's musicians,

all

selection in considering the composers to be

amount of space each composer
be informed about the

lives

Are selections inade, then,
it

is

to receive.

and works of

in

largely a matter of chance

all

there must be

mentioned and the

Our children cannot
who have composed.

accordance with soine principle, or

which

is

composers are included in a

history or an encyclopedia?

To throw
early 1900's

on

light

were

from 1920 on.

It

this

problem, histories and encyclopedias of the

studied, as well as others published in each decade

was found that the publications, some 22 or more

in each decade, agreed

among themselves

quite well, that

is,

they

devoted almost the same relative amounts of space to the musicologists'

favored

92

composers. ^9 The coefficients of correlation

ranged from -94 to -96, demonstrating considerable communality of

136

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
judgment. These high correlations along with the others reported
earlier

show

clearly that musical taste

concern, then,

is

is

not whimsical.

Our next

with the nature of the principle which governs

it.

Allocation of Space
Rank Order

Histories of the

1

2

C

1

930' s

Histories of the

Wagner

Beethoven

Beethoven

Wagner

Mozart

Mozart

4

Bach

Bach

Brahms

3

(

5

Haydn

6

Liszt

7

Handel

8

Schumann
Brahms

9
10

Schubert

f
1

Haydn
Liszt

(^Schubert

Schumann

10

Alusic Encyclopedias

oj the 1930'

of the 1940's

1

Wagner

(

2

Bach

[

3

Beethoven

Mozart

4

Schubert

Liszt

5

Mozart

Bach

6

Schumann

Schubert

7

Debussy

8

Brahms

r

9

Mendelssohn

\^

13-5

Liszt

General Encyclopedias

Kank Order

of the ip30's

9 40'

[Handel

Music Encyclopedias

Kank Order

1

Wagner
Beethoven

Mendelssohn

1

3

Schumann
Brahms
Debussy

General Encyclopedias

of the ip40's

1

Beethoven

Beethoven

2

Bach

Bach

Mozart

3

Wagner

4

Handel

Wagner

S

Schubert

Mendelssohn
Schubert

6

Mozart

7

Schumann

Schumann

8

Mendelssohn

Handel

9

Tchaikovsky

J
[Tchaikovsky

137

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
CHANGES
relative

similar

found,

We

IN SPACE ALLOCATIONS.

have just Seen that the

amounts of space given the eminent composers tended to be

from decade
it

to decade.

But although similarities are to be

should also be noted that these allocations, like other social

judgments, change slowly but perceptibly

vs^ith

time.

The rank order

or eminence (space-attention) for any one decade

is

found to be

most

is

shown by the

like those of the decades nearest

it

in time. This

fact that the correlation values in the table

below generally decrease

in

magnitude from

is

further evidence for the cultural determination of musical taste.

left to right

and from bottom to top. Here, then,

Intercorrelations

in Space

Allocations
Histories

of the

1920's

1930's

1940's

•90

-88

-78

•90

-88

Histories of the 1900's
Histories of the 1920's

•90

Histories of the 1930's

Music Encyclopedias of the

1920's

1930's

1940's

•77

-74

-72

•91

^89

Music Encyclopedias of the 1900's
Music Encyclopedias of the 1920's

Music Encyclopedias of the 1930's

•95

General Encyclopedias oj the

1920's

1930's

•90

-88

-88

•94

^91

General Encyclopedias of the 1900's
General Encyclopedias of the 1920's

1940's

•93

General Encyclopedias of the 1930's

Individual and Group Dijerences
In the earlier portion of this chapter, the similarities rather than

the differences

among

aesthetic responses

an emphasis does not need to be justified,

were

we

any given social pressure does not stimulate
138

stressed.

While such

should not forget that
all

people in the same

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
way. That

is,

even

musical taste obeys social principles, individual

if

differences of considerable magnitude will of necessity be present in

they are in

this behavioral area, just as

realms of

who

human

respond to

passivity

In any social

response.

other socially significant

all

group there will be those

a particular social pressure

with almost complete

Others will be more

and compliance.

resistant,

and

a

few

will be extremely unconventional.

Although the pooling of items

is

necessary

if

trends are to be

established,

this

process tends to mask whatever differences are

present, and

may

give an erroneous impression of the extent of the

agreement between people, books, programs, or whatever the items

may

be.

To

avoid

all

possibility of misinterpretation,

to study the diversity of the elements

To

illustrate this process,

were presented,
since i860.

late in

might be wise

it

which make up

a typical pool.

126 randomly selected college students

1946, with a

They were asked

list

to rank

on

of over 200 composers born
a scale

of 10 to

i

the names

of those they regarded as the ten leading composers. The response

were divided by simple

sheets

A

labeled

The

alternation into

two equal

and B.

table

shows the detailed distribution of the votes

of the twelve composers

The number

who

received the greatest

cast for each

number

of votes.

top of each column shows the ascribed rank, and

at the

the numbers below (opposite the names of the composers)

number

piles,

show the

of votes cast for each composer under each rank. Thus,

twelve of the students in Pool
rating of 9,

etc.

To

A

gave Debussy a rating of 10,

five a

number of

votes

figure the final ranks,

the

received by each composer was multiplied by the respective rankvalue, the products

were

totaled,

and the sums so obtained were

arranged in order of size and ranked.
figuring

times

proceeded

8, etc.

Note

as follows:

12 times 10 plus g times 9 plus 12

that even Debussy,

in each pool, received a

For Debussy (Pool A) the

number

who

of 4-,

achieved the top position

3-,

2-,

and

i

-order votes.

139

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
Thus the examination of the inner structure of the two pools reveals

enormous

among

individual differences

the judgments of the raters.

a

view of the two rank orders shows almost unanimity of opinion

when

the voters are considered as groups rather than as individuals.

Yet

Pool A
OF Choice

O RDER
10

9

8

7

6

5

4

3

2

1

Sums

Debussy

12

S

12

5

4

3

2

3

1

364

1

Kreisler

1

3

4
4

2

3

4

2

4

1

120

11

Sibelius

7

7

9

6

1

2

6

2

4

297

3

Ravel

1

8

3

2

2

4

4

2

176

9

R. Strauss

7

8

3

5

4

4

3

3

1

268

5

Paderewski

4

2

6

2

2

5

4

1

3

4

186

7

3

1

4

7

2

2

2

1

1

132

10

Composer

Prokofiev

1

Rank

Rachmaninov

7

12

6

11

5

2

1

3

2

358

2

Shostakovich

6

4

4

4

8

5

3

1

2

3

251

6

Stravinsky

2

2

3

5

5

1

4

8

4

3

Gershwin

7

6

7

1

4

5

3

4

3

5

1

I

4

4

1

3

2

3

2

1

Sums

Schuman,

W.

i

183

8

271

4

60

12

Pool B

O RDER OF Choice
Composer

Debussy

6

5

4

5

1

5

3

3

2

1

395

1

1

4

1

5

5

5

134

11

276

5

3

260

7

296

4

10

9

8

7

16

7

10

4

Kreisler

1

1

Sibelius

7

7

5

5

2

4

2

4
4

8

6

S

8

4

5

2

4

Ravel
9

R. Strauss

Paderewski
Prokofiev

1

7

5

5

2

4

2

4

8

8

7

6

3

1

1

6

5

3

6

7

3

1

2

2

3

14

8

8

4

10

2

3

2

5

5

3

5

6

5

9

3

Stravinsky

2

7

2

4

2

8

7

Gershwin

9

3

S

9

7

6

7

2

1

1

1

1

2

Rachmaninov
Shostakovich

Schuman,
It

W.

has just been

8

shown

4

229

8

177

10

1

393

2

1

2

264

6

2

5

2

225

9

3

2

1

334

3

3

4

3

72

12

that individual musical tastes differ

do other individual behaviors governed by

Rani

social pressures.

much

as

More-

over, characteristic differences as well as similarities also can be

140

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
detected

among

orchestras.

In

symphony

the programs of the leading American
their

of orchestral

study

careful

idiosyncracies,

Mueller and Hevner have given us a w^ealth of information, from

which are presented here
program

a

few

illustrations of the characteristic

have disclosed. 3^

styles their researches

During its years under Koussevitzky the Boston Symphony Orchestra
,

changed from lowest to highest place in the amount of Russian music
it

presented. But this great organization has never, except for a five-

year period under the batons of French conductors, favored French

music. Moreover,
prisingly

it

has given little attention to

enough when

its

foreign-born personnel

Wagner. And,
is

considered,

always been a proponent of American music Since 1920
.

outstanding leader in the presentation of music of the

it

Philharmonic Orchestra have reflected

it

has

has been an

modern period.

the programs of the

Since the time of Stransky,

sur-

less interest in

New

York

Russian music

than have the programs of the other leading American orchestras.

But they have indicated more than average interest in German music,
an interest held to throughout

World War

I.

French and American

music, on the other hand, have been rather neglected. The program
trends of the sister organization, the
are the

most irregular and

The programs of

this

orchestra have been

American music, but strong

The programs of

weak

in

German and

in French.

the Philadelphia Orchestra have reflected almost

an average interest in the music of
ties,

New York Symphony Orchestra,

difficult to characterize of the entire study.

with the exception of the

interest indicated for the

all

the chief Occidental nationali-

British.

There

is

less

than average

music of Beethoven, Mozart, Mendelssohn,

Handel (except from 192^ to 1930), Schumann, DTndy,
Rachmaninov (except from 191^ to 1920, 1925^ to 1930, and 1937

Liszt,

to 1940), Saint-Saens, Berlioz, and Glazunov.

average attention has been paid

to

In contrast,

more than

Haydn, Bach (up to

1936),

Brahms, Wagner, Debussy, Stravinsky, Sibelius, and Mahler.
141

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
The opera
two

these

in

New York

and Chicago also has been scrutinized by

researchers, with the result that certain characteristics for

each geographic area have been uncovered. This research on the

opera companies and that on the orchestras make
the hierarchy of composer prestige

it

clear that while

roughly similar from one

is

organization to another, conductors, directors, critics, and perhaps
others stamp their individualities on the programs.

Criteria

With

and Conditioners of Taste

C.

Seashore's dictum that beauty consists in artistic

E.

the regular or rigid no

deviations from

illustrations of this

phenomenon

one need quarrel, for

are legion in

all

of the

arts. 3^

The

painter never paints with completely photographic accuracy; the
vocalist

and the

regular

known

violinist
as

make

great use of that deviation from the

But

the vibrato.

if

one should desire to make

specific use of this generalization in forecasting

positions will be accepted and
it

of

little

help.

generation

For what

may not be

a deviation

is

which

will be rejected,

considered an

to

some

he will find

artistic deviation in

so regarded in another.

must be acceptable

which future com-

To be

one

called artistic,

particular culture group.

And the reasons for its acceptance may be difficult to ascertain.
Howes and others have maintained that taste in music is good
whenever one chooses the sincere; avoids the shallow, the
mental, and the cliche; and

emotions. 32

comes

Some

A

not deceived by base motives and

writers have implied that

to appreciate

develops good

is

fact,

142

the listener

and so eventually

taste.

shows that no one can ever

be absolutely certain of a composer's motives
life,

somehow

the composer's motives,

careful examination of this position

of his

senti-

and certainly not from

a

mere

—not even from

a study

scrutiny of his music.

the musical genius often does not understand his

own

In

impulses,

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
and

asked about them will answer with rationalizations of the most

if

unrealistic sort.

hoped

Was Wagner's music

democratic ideals?

Is

we

have with

who

are not of much help to one

And

as for

Wagner

his earlier

boogiewoogie shallow and sentimental per

or because of the associations
clear,

insincere because

would bring him fame, and because he forgot

it

the cliche,

it

it?

These terms,

se,

it is

desires to study taste.

need only be

said that

while there

is

a

tendency to eschew the hackneyed, cliches are matters of personal

may become

experience, and the cliche of one period

music
at

in the

next (and vice versa). Thus, parallel

one time, but became cliches

second period brought on

To

lifted.

a

is

only

now

this

being gradually

the listener born in this century, parallel intervals do not

constitute hackneyed material but
strange.

acceptable

were popular

Their excessive use in

later.

ban which

fifths

Thus Howes's

criteria,

In considering the

we

as alluring

it

and

are forced to conclude, are too

Only the cliche has

subjective for our purposes.

explanatory concept, and even

may be regarded

utility

an

as

has very definite limitations.

phenomena which

are basic to the appreciation

of music, E. H. Staffelbach has stressed the feeling tone he believes
to be implicit in rhythms and auditory sensations; the associations

formed between music and persons,

places, things,

and previously

expressed affective states; the lure of the familiar and the novel; the
stimulation of the imagination; the possibilities of self-expression;

and the pleasures to be derived from the genius and good craftsmanship of composers and performers,

works of these
what

artists,

artists are

from

facts

about the

and from the belief in one's

attempting to express.

33

lives

and

ability to interpret

C. E. Seashore, a pioneer

in the scientific study of music, has said:

Why
it

then do

because

it is

built

selves;

it

it

we

love music?

Among

other things

we

love

creates a physiological well-being in our organism;

from materials which are beautiful objects
carries us

in

them-

through the realms of creative imagination,
143

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
thought, actions, and feelings in limitless art forms;

it is self-

propelling through natural impulses, such as rhythm;

language of emotion, a generator of social fellowship;
us out of the
ideal;

it

isolation

humdrum

life

and makes us

live in play

it

the

takes

with the

our cravings for intellectual conquest, for

satisfies

in

of

is

it

the

of emotion,

attitude

artistic

and for

self-

expression for the joy of expression. 34

Whether or not we agree with
reasons for the love of music,
further review.
theories

It

is

it

all

of these carefully considered

should be clear that the topic needs

time, then, to examine in

which purport

some

to explain the reasons behind

current preferences. The facts of the situation are

still

detail the

some of our

quite unclear.

But sufficient data have already been assembled to prove the great

importance of both formal training and incidental learning

in the

building up of taste norms.

CULTURAL DERIVATIONS.

If taste is

Culturally derived rather

than innate, one would expect to find the taste of the child approxi-

mating more and more closely that of the adult

Such

a

as

he grows older.

convergence does occur in judgments of eminence.

In

one

study in which several groups had been asked to assess the relative

eminence of 92 composers,
only slightly with the
(a rho

of '3 3).

But

a

the musicologists in

more marked

for

a

group of fifth-graders was found to agree

members

of the American Musicological Society

group of sixth-graders was somewhat closer to
its

judgements

(rho of '^2); the similarity

some high-school students

Stanford University sophomores ('79).

(•68), and

still

more

was
for

Musicologists surveyed in

19^1 and college students polled in 195^4 agreed to the extent of a
rho of -77.

Here we see

the groups

become more

In

1

9

1

3

a steady

as

and in training.

Valentine reported on some of his experiments with

British school children. 35

144

climb in the degree of agreement

alike in age

He found

that by age twelve or thirteen

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
there tended to be a decided change in the preferences for tonal

by adults w^ere

intervals, so that the intervals preferred

Another psychologist,

by adolescents.

Dashiell,

also

enjoyed

observed that,

although kindergarten children did not have precisely the adult
preferences for the diatonic intervals, they already rated the thirds

high and the sevenths and seconds low.^^ Aizawa,

who had examined

the songs most enjoyed by Japanese school children, noted that

agreements

and adults increased

in preference of children

school year advanced, being particularly marked

among

as

the

children of

the upper classes. 37

The Kwalwasser-Dykema

of Melodic Taste, in

Test

which 20 items

are considered for their suitability as concluding phrases, can be

employed

yield data

to

While the college

which

reflect

the learning of folkways.

subjects in one study of this test

with the "correct" responses on

showed agreement

out of 20 items, eighth-graders

i£'4.

gave the "correct" responses 13 times, and fifth-graders only i2'2
times on the average.
of Harmonic Sensitivitj

Similarly,

when

were analyzed,

data of the Kwalwasser Test

was found that an unselected

it

group of college students averaged 2^*4 "correct" scores out of
a possible

ages

eighth-graders 24-1, and fifth-graders 2i'2.

35",

became more

similar the responses

became more

As the

like those of

more like the "correct" responses.
In the areas of tempo and rhythm, it is noteworthy that subjects
show consistent preferences, but differ considerably among them-

the experts,

selves. 3^

i.e.

Although

consistency"

is

mentioned data

one writer

feels

that

this

the result of inheritance, 39 there are the previously
(p. 9)

which

show

clearly

that

are in part functions of cultural conditioning.
recalled,

proceeds

"intra-individual

tempo preferences
Foley,

it

will be

found that subjects studying trades in which activity
at a

slow pace,

those working with

preferred a

e.g.,

dressmaking, favored andante tempo;

power machines,

fast allegro

a

slow allegro; while

his typists

bordering on presto. "^^ Other experimenters

14^

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
(p.

69) have found that college students prefer their waltzes to be

played at about the speed they have heard them rendered traditionally.

In fact, these students have demonstrated a sort of absolute

or positive tempo quite comparable to absolute pitch.
Somevs^hat similar to the w^ork of Foley, but broader in

was the study by Schuessler.^^

its

scope,

Eight phonograph recordings w^ere

played to large groups of subjects

who had been

occupation into six job

On

levels.

which Schuessler employed, great

divided according to

the live-point preference scale

differences of scores appeared.

CLIMATIC CYCLES AND TASTE.

According to one of the

climatic determinists, R. H. Wheeler, world climate fluctuates "in

rhythms within rhythms which tend to follow multiples of the sunspot cycle of II'

years. "4^

3

Whether long or

short, these

rhythms

"tend to follow a similar pattern of phase sequences, the phases

being cold-dry, warm-wet, warm-dry, cold-wet, then cold-dry again,
in that order."

Phases of

warm weather

are supposed to bring into

being musical taste for (and so creativity in the area of) the serious
opera, the symphony, the sonata, the concerto,

chamber music,

swing, jazz syncopation, masses, anthems, oratorios, dissonance, and
atonality.

Warm

weather

is

also said to

Cold

choruses, orchestras, and bands.

somehow put
operas,

be conducive to the
spells,

rise

of

on the other hand,

us temperamentally in tune with light and comic

program music, tone poems,

and counterpoint

—according

to the

folk music, ballads, madrigals,

Wheeler doctrine. During cold

periods occur the heyday of Castrati and musicology

"golden ages" of music were neither

warm nor

!

However, the

cold, but

were rather

periods of transition from cold to warm.

Wheeler holds

that

we

are

now

in a

cold-wet phase which

not leave until 2000 or 2010 except for
this

decade or the

1830; the

146

last

1

960's.

warm

Our

last

a ten-year

warm

we

will

period in

previous cold phase began around

period about 1900.

Since such swings are held

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
we need merely

by Wheeler to be the rule of nature, he beUeves that

to study weather cycles to forecast the taste of any future time period.

The only escape from
says, to

No

would seem, he

these predetermined swings

be in universal air-conditioning.

scientist

would dare

say that the

eventually be found to have

some very

surprising, to say the least,

if

changes the

vs^orld has

Wheeler doctrine may not

slight validity.

Yet

it

would be

the relatively insignificant temperature

endured during the past

few^ centuries should

have caused huge changes in aesthetic interests and taste while the

Minnesota and California are

far greater climatic differences of, say,

apparently unrelated to musical taste.

TASTE CREATED BY THE STATE.

It

is

popular to believe that good taste in music

becoming increasingly
is

indicated by special

Some

reverence for those compositions which mirror the times.

would go

so far as to say that

that type of music

"good" causes.

which has

Ordinarily,

good
a

taste insures the

honoring of only

propaganda value for the furthering of

little is

done to promote the acceptance

of compositions which are so regarded. In lands under police control,

however,
positions

this

doctrine

which are

is

often so rigidly enforced that only

felt to

com-

mirror the times or which follow the

state's

approved pattern can be heard. 43 The

fact that the leaders

may be

deluding themselves

or propaganda worth

as to the timeliness

of their approved compositions

beside the point.

is

musical works can be heard, and

it is

healthier to

to laugh at them, they will quite likely

masses. 44

This

TRAINING.

is

one way to create

Little

by

little,

come

to

If

only certain

honor these than

be accepted by the

taste.

data are being accumulated

which

demonstrate the potency of both auditory and visual training in the
creation of musical taste. 45 As Tyler 4^ and C. L. Stone 47 have shown,

we

are trained to think of music in terms of stereotypes.

Even the
147

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
been taught to

relatively unsophisticated college student has

names of Mozart, Beethoven, and Schubert
position.

music he associates with the

Italian

The

chapters also

airy,

German music

studies reported in previous

of training in stereotyped thinkina.

tell

the

com-

and the

light

Russian music w^ith the somber and the gloomy, and

with the heavy and the philosophic.

tie

to certain styles of

For American

grade-school children surely have few facts to go on

when

Beethoven and Bach so high on their eminence

They answer not

from deep conviction, but rather

The

situation

Lincoln
J.

Stone

and

4^

W.

lists

Robinson

war music.

War, the

lists.

accordance with their teaching.

analogous to their placing of Jesus, Washington, and

the top of their

at

aspects of
Civil

is

in

of the most eminent
49

of

all

time.

have studied the sociopsychological

soldiers' songs reflected their

common

traditions and

Although noting that each war to some

extent mirrors the times, Robinson stresses the

wars sing about: bad food, insect

all

men

Stone points out that during the American

not their war alignments.

soldiers of

they place

common

subjects

war

terrors,

pests,

the enemy, sweethearts and other loved ones, and the desire to return

home.
Gardner and Pickford have demonstrated by experiment what
absolutists have difficulty in understanding: that perceived dissonance
varies

with (i) the listener's experience, training, and traditions,

and (2) the musical "intent" of the passage

as a


(3) the physical composition of the chord.

listener's recent experiences

experiment
L' Histoire

in

du

sophomores.

on enjoyment has

which Beethoven's

Sold at

Fifth

were presented

to

whole,

The

also

as

well as

effect

of the

been shown

Symphony and Stravinsky's
several groups of college

When the playing of the Beethoven composition preceded

that of the Stravinsky, the professed enjoyment-ratings

and 27'4 respectively
enjoyment").

When

for the Stravinsky

148

in an

(a

were 73-8

rating of 100 equaled "greatest possible

the order was reversed, the values

number, and

79-

1

became

for Beethoven's Fifth.

34*5^

Note the

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
when it followed the better
latter when it was played second.

depressive effect on the less enjoyed
liked,

and the enhancement of the

Wiebe found

that the extensive playing of popular songs over the

radio did not appear to increase his student subjects' liking for them.^^

However, lack of "plugging" seemed
preference.

WNYC

have done

much

in a decrease

result

of

that the broadcasts of radio station

to develop an interest in

music among

its

and to establish taste norms. 5^ The broadcasts make music

listeners

more

Suchman concluded

to

accessible; they extend the range of musical experience; they

repeat the musical stimuli; they supply commentators and, on occasion,

program notes53 and other educational

liability

ment

which may be inherent

A

literature.

in radio taste training

is

possible

the develop-

of a dependent attitude of listening attentively only to

positions

com-

which are sponsored by the broadcasters of the favored

radio station.

A number

of researchers have attempted to telescope history by

forcing their subjects to hear the same musical stimuli

within a relatively short period.
several studies are not in

54

many times

Although the conclusions of the

complete agreement,

that a composition with little variety reaches

seems

safe to say

maximum

acceptance

it

quickly, and then as speedily declines in popularity.

Music with

more complexity tends to gain acceptance more slowly and to
become hackneyed less rapidly. In one paper it has been suggested
that the continuous repetition of musical material has a stronger
effect

on the

listener than

hearings. 55 Mull has

made

would the same number of well-spaced

the interesting discovery that the preferred

parts of musical compositions tend to

Since the spread of preference usually

become
is

larger

greater at the anterior end

of the focal region, Mull has suggested that this
a case of

"Might

it

what psychologists

on rehearing. 5^

phenomenon may be

call "goal gradient."

As she phrases

it:

not be that learning to like a composition has features

similar to those involved in an animal's learning to run a

maze



that

149

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
is

in the case of

to say,

our experiment, an original high spot

(corresponding to the maze goal)

may be

thereafter anticipated and

extend backward? Thus, pleasure would actually

a pleasure gradient

spread from a focus, rather than appear de novo.
reached, there would be relaxation of interest

Once the climax is
and some tendency

for the pleasure to drop off."

A

peculiar situation has developed in the area of phonograph and

radio listening, in

which tones of relatively "poor"

quality, i.e.,

timbres unlike those of the "live" instruments, are

at

with

present pre-

ferred to those of more realistic quality. Apparently, long-continued,

informal training

the responsible agent.

is

set of taste habits for listening to the

another to function

when

We

have developed one

phonograph and the

in the presence of orchestral instruments. 57

Chinn and Eisenberg, two of the investigators of
have shown that

this

type of preference

is

this

phenomenon,

found among the musically

sophisticated as well as the naive, and persists even
is

radio, and

when

the listener

told that a wider tonal band yields tones closer to the sounds of

real life. 5^

Until recently the lay preference has been for an un-

usually strong emphasis

from the

on the

bass.

Presumably

fact that the engineers early

this

preference arose

achieved reasonable realism in

the lower tonal ranges but have only recently brought in the very

high frequencies.

As

a

matter of

fact,

the early attempts of

RCA

Victor to produce "high fidelity" led to such poor audience response
that this manufacturer continued for
bass.

However, with "high

lovers,

years with an overloaded

now

an essential for

all

music

one might guess that there will shortly appear an equally

extreme

The

fidelity"

some

taste for

an overloaded treble.

Hi-Fi Hall, with

its

incredibly short reverberation time,

necessitates further taste adjustments. 59

Long accustomed

deadening effects of most auditoriums, the listener
to react to greater brilliance, to tone that

While the

hi-fi

is

is

to the

here called upon

"acoustically naked."

enthusiast has little difficulty in learning to appreciate

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
persons

this "drier" effect,
^

somewhat

at first

Some
can

call

more attuned

more

up than to the music

said to

^'^

Schoen terms

if

there

is

its

this

group into

great concern with the objective features of

moods,

the attention

if

changes within the listener's

own

is

on

real or apparent

body, and "character,"

if

the

music

itself.

divides this group into "sensorial" and "perceptual."

While

listener imputes

these classifications

may

traits,

to

and

activities to the

some readers look

suspiciously like fixed

clearly not the intent of these theorists to create typo-

types,

it is

logies.

They

are, rather,

emphasizing the different things people have

learned to "see" in music and they realize
listeners

sort

Such persons are often

have taste of a high order. Myers subdivides

Ortmann

extreme

Myers "associative,"^^ and Ortmann

calls "intrinsic" listeners.

the music, "intrasubjective,"

We

this

Those who pay more attention to the music than to

Schoen

"objective,"

sets are

attention to the associations music

itself.

of response "extrinsic" listening,
"imaginal."^^

home

startled, to say the least.

of us learn to pay

associations

to the older

full

well that few

if

any

belong to a single category.

hardly need proof to be certain that taste develops out of

experiences gained in home, church, club, and school, and out of
contacts with the concert stage, recordings, the radio, and the printed
page. These agencies of education, propaganda, and censorship force

us to revere certain composers and their compositions, and to take less
seriously other

men and their works. We come to have several standards

of taste: for the concert stage, for the dance hall, ^3 for church, and for

school



to

mention some of the more important. Age,

and special training are important variables
formation. ^4 But

it is

difficult to

be

specific

intelligence,

in this process of taste

about

all this

since there

seems to be a great difference between taste as it is observed in everyday
life

If

and the sort of

we

taste

people are willing to admit they possess. ^5

take as our aim the inculcation in our children of the

standards of taste that adults of our culture regard as "good," checks
^S^

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
on the success of

endeavor with groups of children can be made

this

through the use oftaste and attitudinal tests, interviews, and operational
methodologies. Care should be taken, however, that the standards
set

up are not made too narrow, and

absolute and unchangeable.

toward the widening of the

For

that they are not thought of as

training

if taste

is

to be directed

enjoyment of music,

possibilities for the

it

would seem obvious that a standard of taste which embraces only a few
composers of top eminence will be

less effective

standard which leads to an interest in

many

than a

styles of

more

catholic

composition.

Summary
The

first thesis

considered in this chapter was,

matter of whimsey or
clear



taste

is

it

is

lawful.

It

in

"Is

musical taste a

some way lawful?" The answer was

was noted that the musically

have

elite

surprisingly similar tastes in their selections of eminent composers.

Even more surprising
exist

among

shared,
elite

also

and the
as

the high agreement on eminence found to

lay public alike.

forming

at least

must be mentioned

And,

if

knowledge of composers

one facet of musical

as lawful,

taste,

is

of other similarly chosen samples.

is

knowledge

man known well
be equally well known

for a

segment of the population tends to

members

Enjoyment, too,

the music of certain composers given top billing by the

vs^ith

accepted

is

college and high-school groups.

to

one

to the

Analyses of musicians'

nonverbal behavior further attest to the fact that musical taste

is

not

whimsical. Thus, the composers whose works are most recorded

and appear most often on the programs of symphony orchestras
with few exceptions, those regarded
they are likely to be the

as

most eminent.

men whose biographical

are,

Moreover,

sketches occupy most

space in histories of music and in general and music encyclopedias.

The second question

the chapter posed was that of learning whether

the laws oftaste are absolute or relative.

1^2

Here, too, the answer was

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
unequivocal. Taste follows no absolute, metaphysical rules.

even
as

if

they

among

natural science variables are

may

well be



the determiners of taste

must be

their importance

And

slight at best.

All

the evidence so far gathered points rather to the rejatiyity of taste,
to the fact that

it is

culture-bound, not culture-free. The descrip-

tions sociology gives of

mores

most

taste

fit

Change with

exactly.

'

training and individual differences in the acceptance of standards,

both characteristics of musical
folkways.

The teaching of

taste,

are earmarks of

then,

taste,

is

mores and

essentially a process of

indoctrination, and the material to be learned differs

somewhat from

culture to culture and from period to period.

Notes
i]

D. D. Runes, ed., The Dictionary of Philosophy, N.Y., Philosophical Library, 1942.

2]

M.

F.

Meyer, The Musician's

Arithmetic, p.

3]

Ibid., p. 81.

4]

P. R. Farnsworth, "Aesthetic Behavior

109.

and Astrology," Char, and

Person.,

6 (1938):

33J-340S]

The reader who enjoys

interesting articles by

speculations based on physics and neurology

N. Rashevsky, "Suggestions for

a

is

referred to the

Mathematical Biophysics of

Auditory Perception with Special Reference to the Theory of Aesthetic Ratings of

Combinations of Musical Tones," Bull. Math. Biophjsics, 4 (1942): 27-32; "An Alternate
Approach to the Mathematical Biophysics of Perception of Combinations of Musical
Tones," op.

cit.,

pp. 89—90.

6]

See Chapter 4 of A. Einstein's Greatness

7]

P.

in Music,

N.Y., Oxford U. Press, 1941.

R. Farnsworth, "The Musical Taste of an American Musical Elite," Hinrichsen's

Musical Year Book, 7 (19^2): 11 2— 116.
8]

P.

R. Farnsworth, "Stereotypes in the Field of Musical Eminence," Genet. Psychol.

Monog., 24 (1941): 347-381; "Musical Eminence," Sch. and Soc, £0 (1939): 1^8-160.
9]
I

o]

P. R. Farnsworth, Musical Taste, Stanford, Calif., Stanford

When it is

ments, each

is

U. Press,

desired to find the degree of correspondence between

ranked and an index of relationship

is

i9i'o.

two

figured. This value

sets of

may

measure-

vary from

I* 00 (which shows the rank orders to be identically arranged) through o (where there is
no correspondence) to — i-oo (where the two rank orders are inversely arranged; the
highest name in one array is lowest in the other). The rho of -97 indicates that the two

1^3

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
piles

were ordered very

regarded
1

1]

Unless otherwise designated the

similarly.

Unless otherwise stated,

it is

to be

method was employed, and

1938 survey formed the

list

2]

3]

all

to be

that the 92

names mentioned

in the

of items to be correlated.

names are offered with no thought that the number nine
The number is kept constant only for the sake of uniformity.

In these tables, the nine top

has any particular significance.
I

are

assumed that wherever coefficients of correlation

are mentioned the rank

1

r/io's

as positive.

For a discussion of

how

jazz experts regard the

music, see the annual polls in Down Beat (the

first

contemporary performers of

jazz

of which was in 195^3), or Metronome'

"All-Star" polls.
14]

R. Farnsworth, "Musical Eminence and Year of Birth," J. Aesth., 4 (1945):

P.

107—109.
I

j]

When

the birth years of a group of composers are arranged in order from

remote to most recent, the middle birth year

is

any other contains birth dates of composers

the mode.

16]

is

the median. That year

most

which more than

These data were gathered through the courtesy of Dr R. Granneberg, of the San

Francisco City College.
17]

Alfred

Chronicle,

Frankenstein,

February

"What People Think They Should Think," San
During the spring of 1947 A.

1941.

2,

F. Michaelis, the

Francisco

program

manager of the (California) Standard Oil Company's "The Standard Hour," conducted a
survey

among

the listeners to this regular

NBC

Sunday evening broadcast. The 8000

who

returned their questionnaires voted Beethoven the most popular composer. The next

few

in

descending order of popular regard were Tchaikovsky,

Wagner, and Johann
Gershwin's Rhapsodj

Strauss.
in Blue.

by Tchaikovsky's Nutcracker
18]

E.

Chopin,

Suite

and Chopin's Polonaise

Militaire.

E. E. Folgmann, "An Experimental Study of Composer- Preference of Four

Outstanding Symphony Orchestras,"
19]

Gershwin,

The most frequently requested of all compositions was
Beethoven's Fifth Symphony came a close second, followed

y. Exp. Psychol.,

16 (1933): 709-724.

H. K. Mull, "Preferred Regions in Music Compositions and the Effect of Repetition

J. Psjchol., S3 (1940): 5^83-^:86; G. Marill and H. K. Mull, "A
Further Study of Preferred Regions in Musical Compositions and the Effect of Repetition

upon Them," Amer.

upon Them,"
20]

W.

S.

y4mer. J. Psjchol.,

Foster and

SS ('942): iio-iii.

M. A. Tinker,

Experiments in Psychology, rev. ed., N.Y., Holt, 1929,

p. 316.

21]

P.

R. Farnsworth, "Studies in the Psychology of Tone and Music," Genet. Psjchol.

Monog., IS, No.

i

(1934): 24-30.

See also O.

W.

Eagleson and L. E. Taylor,

Group of Negro Women," J. Exp.
and "The Preference of Twenty-five Negro College Women
of Chord Preference in a

J. Exp. Psjchol.,

22]

i

28 (1941): 439-442.

P. R. Farnsworth,

"An

Kwalwasser Test Battery,"

1^4

"A Study

26 (1940): 619—62
for Major and Minor Chords,"

Psychol.,

Historical, Critical, and Experimental Study of the SeashoreGenet. Psjchol. Monog., 9,

No.

^ (193 i):

291—393.

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
P. R. Farnsworth, "Studies in the

23]

Monog., IS, No.

The

24]

subjects

Psychology of Tone and Music," Genet. Psychol.

(1934): 50-84.

i

were asked

to

check

all

names they were certain they recognized. The

number of

scoring was the simple procedure of tabulating the
25]

votes each

name

received.

H. Mueller and Kate Hevner, "Trends in Musical Taste," Indiana U. Publ.,

J.

Humanity

Ser.,

See also

Press.

No.
J.

8

(1942), p. 59. Reproduced by permission of the Indiana University

H. Mueller's The American Symphony

Orchestra, a Social History of Musical

Bloomington, Indiana U. Press, 1951, and D. Nash's "The Construction of the

Taste,

Repertoire of a Symphony Orchestra," Thesis, U. of Washington, 1950.

These broadcast programs were obtained through the courtesy of R. R. Gros, an

26]

official

27]

of the company.

J.

H. Mueller and Kate Hevner,

op.

cit.

28] J. H. Mueller, "Methods of Measurement of Aesthetic Folkways," Amer. J. Social., 57,
No. 4 (1946): 276—282. See also the more recent orchestral data in Chapter 4 of Mueller's

The American Sjmphonj Orchestra.

Kaplan, "Telopractice:

For data on social roles within the orchestra see M.

A Symphony

Orchestra

as

It

Prepares for a Concert," Soc. Forces,

33 {^9SS)- 3S^-iSSIn the case of certain of the histories

29]

where the space technique could not be
number of page mentions the composer

followed, the score was based on the relative
received.
30]

J.

H. Mueller and Kate Hevner, "Trends

Musical Taste."

in

Further data can be

obtained in Mueller's article, "Methods of Measurement of Aesthetic Folkways."
C. E. Seashore, Psychology oj Music, N.Y., McGraw-Hill, 1938, p. 267.

31]

According

to L. Vernon, pianists play at least one-half of their chords asynchronously ("Synchronization of Chords in Artistic Piano Music," U. of

W.

See also

la. Stud. Psychol.

H. Lichte's "One Man's Preferred

Mus., 4 (1936): 306-345).

Fifth," Amer. J.

Psychol.,

68 (1955):

312-315.
32]

F.

33]

E.

Howes, The Borderland of Music and

Psychology,

London, Kegan Paul, 1926.

H. Staffelbach, "The Psychology of Music Appreciation," School Musician,

2<)

(1928): 9-13.
34]

C. E. Seashore,

35]

C.

W.

and Adults,"
36]

J. F.

Why We

Love Music, Philadelphia, Ditson, 1941, p. 9.

Valentine, "The Aesthetic Appreciation of Musical Intervals
Brit. J. Psychol.,

among Children

6 (1913): 190-216.

Dashiell, "Children's Sense of

Harmonies

in

Colors and Tones," J. Exp. Psychol.,

2 (1917): 466-47^37]
1 1

M. Aizawa, "The Musical

38]

T.

W.

Harrell, "Factors Influencing Preference and

J. Gen. Psychol., IJ (1937):

39]
1

Taste of School Children," Tohoku Psychol. Folia, 6 (1938):

1-126.

I.

Memory

for Auditory

Rhythm,"

63-104.

Frischeisen-Kohler, "The Personal

Tempo

and

Its

Inheritance," Char, and Person.,

(1933): 301-313-

^55

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
40]
/.

Foley

J. P.

41]

K.

Jr.,

"The Occupational Conditioning of

12 (1940):

Soc. Psychol.,

I

Background and Musical Taste," Amer.

F. Schuessler, "Social

330—33^. For

Preferential Auditory

Tempo,"

21-129.

the preference for popular music during 1942, see

Music," in Radio Research 1^42-43, P.

J.

Social. Rev.,

13 (1948):

and urban-rural differences in

a study of age, sex, regional, educational

G. Peatman, "Radio and Popular

Lazarsfeld and F. Stanton, eds., N.Y., Duell,

F.

Sloan and Pearce, 1944.
42]

R. H. Wheeler, "Climate and

Human

Behavior," in P.

L.

Harriman, ed..

The

N.Y., Philosophical Library, 1946, pp. 78-86; R. H. Wheeler
and T. Gaston, "The History of Music in Relation to Climatic and Cultural Fluctuations,"
Encyclopedia of Psychology

in Mus.

43]

,

Teach. Nat. Assoc. Proc. 1940, pp.

An example

number

"advice" given a

432-438.

of a governmental determination of taste can be seen in the
of the leading composers of the

USSR, February

lo,

official

1948.

pronouncement and the composers' answ^ering statethe American Russian Institute's pamphlet, On Soviet Music, published in

Translations of the governmental

ments appear

May
44]

in

1948, in Hollywood.

The

task of an officially constituted

committee on musical standards

the tendency of the masses to accept what the "experts" claim

experiment on the

effect of expert opinion

on musical

M.

J.

Keston and

easier

by

H. T. Moore, "The

Psychol.,

32 (192

1):

16—20.

Pinto, "Possible Factors Influencing Musical Preference,"

"An Exploratory Study

L. E. Tyler,

Psychol.,

M.

made

86 (19^5): loi — 113.

J. Genet. Psychol.,

46]

I.

is

"good music." For an

taste see

Comparative Influence of Majority and Expert Opinion," y4mer. J.
4^]

is

of Discrimination of

Composer

Style," J. Gen.

34 (1946): 153-163.

47]

C. L. Stone, "Identification of Nationality in Music," Psychol. Bull., 35 (1937): 7^6.

48]

J.

Stone,

"War Music and War Psychology

in the Civil

War,"y. Abn.

Soc. Psychol.,

36

(1941): 543-560.
49]

W.

U. of
50]

Robinson,

Calif. Press,

P. A.

"War

Songs of America," in Writers Congress, Proceedings, Berkeley,

1944, pp. 284-304.

Gardner and R.

W.

Pickford, "Relation between Dissonance and Context,"

For a study of the

Nature (London), 1^4 (1944): 274-275.
playing Bach and jazz in the

effects

same program see G. D. Williams, "The

Appearance on the Appreciation of Musical Selections,"

J.

Gen.

on appreciation of
Effect of

Psychol.,

Order of

2J (1942):

295-310.
51]

G. Wiebe, "The Effect of Radio Plugging on Students' Opinions of Popular Songs,"

]. Appl. Psychol.,

52]

E. A.

24 (1940): 721-727.

Suchman, "Invitation

to Music:

by the Radio," in Radio Research 1941, P.

A

Study of the Creation of

F. Lazarsfeld

and

New Music

F. Stanton, eds.,

Listeners

N.Y., Duell,

Sloan and Pearce, 1941.
53]

For an experiment on the effectiveness of program notes see G. D. Williams, "The

Effect of

Program Notes on the Enjoyment of Musical

(1943), 261-279.

iS6

Selections," J. Gen. Psjchol., 29

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL TASTE
^4]

H. T. Moore, "The Genetic Aspect of Consonance and Dissonance,"

IJ (1914): 1-68; M.

F.

Washburn

"The

et al.,

Effects of

Psychol. Monog.,

Immediate Repetition on the

Pleasantness or Unpleasantness of Music," in The Ejfects of Music, N.Y., Harcourt, Brace,

1927, Chap. 10; A. R. Gilliland and H. T. Moore, "The Immediate and Long-time Effects
of Classical and Popular Phonograph Selections," J. Appl. Psjchol., 8 (1924): 309-323;
H. E. Krugman, "Affective Response to Music as a Function of Familiarity," J. Ahn. Soc.

38 (1943): 388-392; J. E. Downey and G. E. Knapp, "The Effect on a Musical
Progrannme of Familiarity and of Sequence of Selections," in The Effects of Music, Chap, 2
H. K. Mull, "The Effect of Repetition upon the Enjoyment of Modern Music, "J. Psychol.,
Psjchol.,

i

;

43 (1957): i?5-i62.
£S]

M. Verveer,

E.

"Change

et al.,

in Affectivity

with Repetition,"

Arner. J. Psychol.,

45

(1933): 130-134^6]

H. K. Mull, "Preferred Regions

upon Them," Amer.
conception

as it

in Musical

Compositions and the Effect of Repetition

S3 (1940): 5^83-^86. For further data on the gradient
applies to music, see G. H. S. Razran's "Studies in Configurational
J. Psjchol.,

Conditioning. VII: Ratios and Elements in Salivary Conditioning to Various Musical
Intervals," Psychol. Rec, 2 (1938):

Sj]

This suggestion

acoustic

filter

370—376.

supported by a study by H. F. Olson,

is

who found

that

when

was placed between sound and audience the preference was for

frequency range. ("Frequency Range Preference for Speech and Music,"

an

all-

a full

J. Acoust. Soc.

Amer., 19 (1947): SA9-SSS-)
58]

H. A. Chinn and P. Eisenberg, "Tonal Range and Sound-intensity Preferences of

Broadcast Listeners,"

Radio Eng., Proc, 33 (194^), ^71-581.

Inst.

j9]

"The new sound,"

60]

M. Schoen, "The

Time, 66,

No. 26 {1955): 40.

Aesthetic Attitude in Music," Psjchol. Monog., 3g (1928): 162-184.

See also P. E. Vernon, "The Phenomena of Attention and Visualization in the Psychology
of Musical Appreciation,"
61]

C.

S.

21 (1930): 50-63.

Brit. J. Psjchol.,

Myers, "Individual Differences in Listening to Music,"

Brit. J.

Psychol.,

13

(1922): 52-71.
62]

O. Ortmann, "The Sensorial

Basis

of Music Appreciation," J.

Comp.

Psjchol.,

2

(1922): 227-256.
63]

J.

E.

Eggen, "A Behavioristic Interpretation of Jazz," Psjchol. Rev., 33 (1926)

571-581.
64]

G. Rubin-Rabson, "The Influence of Age, Intelligence, and Training on Reactions to

Classic

and Modern Music,"

J. Gen. Psjchol.,

Experimental Study of the Factors Involved
Thesis,

Columbia U., 193

i

M.

;

J.

Keston,

22 (1940): 413-429; E. G. Plotkin, "An
in the Appreciation of Standard Music,"

"An Experimental Evaluation of the

Efficiency

Two Methods

of Teaching Music Appreciation," J. Exp. Ed., 22 (1954): 215-226. For
an experiment on increasing tolerance with age, see P. R. Farnsworth, "Changes in

of

Musical Taste," J. Musical.,
65]

1

(1939): 1-4.

R. L. Fisher, "Preferences of Different Age and Socio-economic Groups in Un-

structured Musical Situations," J. Soc. Psychol., 33 (1951): 147-152.

iS7

CHAPTER SEVEN

The Measures

We

have seen that

siderable stability.
like other social

of Musical Taste

taste,

Moreover,

A

its
it

several manifestations, has con-

has been

shown

that taste behaves

phenomena of our culture and not

obeying some absolute law.^

ment

in

It is

now

were

as if it

time to consider the measure-

of taste, the problem of the current chapter.

Variety of Measures
Experience has shown that no one measure of musical taste can

hope

to tap the

ensemble or

totality of preferences.

Several pro-

cedures, then, are needed, each to paint a partial picture.

the measures

we

many

are about to describe are of

Though

different types,

they can for convenience be divided into two main categories
(i) the formal tests, and (2) the approaches

such standardized procedures. The

which do not employ

tests, in turn,

auditory and the purely paper-and-pencil types.

can be

The

less

split into

the

standardized

—the

methods include the techniques discussed in the previous chapter

interview and the counting of ballots, the measurement of space
allocations,^

and the analysis of programs

phonograph record

1^8

listings.



as

well as the tabulation of

THE MEASURES OF MUSICAL TASTE
Auditory Tests

One

of the earliest of the standardized music tests was Columbia

record number

A 7^39,

designed by C. E, Seashore as a measure of

the "sense of consonance," rather than

as a test

of taste. 3

However,

the directions proved to be impossible of execution, and the term

"consonance" was seen to be so ambiguous that the

from the

later revision of the Seashore battery.

test

The record

been more suitably employed by some researchers
test for tonal intervals.

was dropped

as a

has since

preference

In giving the test, go sets of simultaneous

dyads are presented in pairs, with the second of each pair to be

judged

as "better"

measure
of

'65"

is,

by

or "worse" than the

first.

As the

reliability of the

a very conservative estimate, only in the

for adults and considerably

be limited to group work. 4
reliability figure,

low

It

lower for children,

neighborhood
its

use should

should be noted, however, that

as it is, at least equals the values

this

reported for a

number of the other tonal appreciation tests.
The early Kwalwasser battery consists of two tests of 35^ items
each. The Melodic Sensitivity Test presents two-measure melodic
progressions, and the Harmonic Sensitivity

progressions of three chords each.

The

Test presents

harmonic

stimuli are to be rated as

"good" or "bad."

Bad melodic progressions result from the following: bad
resolutions, incompatible tones,

awkward rhythms,

failure to

turn after a wide skip, lack of design or purposiveness, distorted
balance, incompleteness of

melody or rhythm,

etc.

Authorities

agree that bad harmonic effects result from parallel
octaves,

wrong

doublings, bad-sounding voice

fifths

and

movement, bad

part omissions, digressions, unprepared modulations, unresolved

dissonances, voice distribution over too

wide

a range, etc.

Scores on these two Kwalwasser tests intercorrelate best at the
fifth-grade level

with

a value of '40.

The corresponding value

for the
i5"9

THE MEASURES OF MUSICAL TASTE
eighth grade
•42

-29, and for college, '24.

is

and -21

(adult level),

these

two

With

reliabilities of

must be rated

tests

only

as

questionable value, except possibly for studying group trends.

of

The

have the disadvantage of offering stimuli which are completely

tests

out of musical context and of scoring the answers on the basis of
rules

other

from

a

past.

To

a greater or lesser extent

many

of the

described below suffer similarly.

tests

The

now

day

Kwalwasser-Dykema battery has among

later

appear to be directly concerned with

The

taste.

its tests

two

that

Melodic Taste Test

measures, "on the basis of general music appeal, sensitiveness to
structure, balance, and phrase compatibility."^ Each test item consists

of two melodies of two phrases each.

two melodies
latter are to

The

are identical, but the

be compared for their

test has only 10 items.

allow the test

The second

much

test

As

this

The opening phrases of the
second ones are unlike. The

suitability as

number

concluding phrases.

of stimuli

reliability, its usefulness

is

is

too small to

definitely limited,

having to do with taste, the measure of Tonal

Movement, offers 30 patterns, each consisting of four tones.
patterns are incomplete melodically, and

mentally a
below

it.

fifth

With

statistically

tone, reporting whether
its

reliability

the best of the

K-D

th,e

it is

above the fourth tone or

for adults in the -So's,

battery.

There

is

One

this

test

tests,

and the

show only chance resemblances.

of the earliest tests of appreciation which employed music

the one by Courtis, in which the child

is

asked to recognize

and rhythmic movements. Typical of the directions

is

160

how

the music says John

felt.^

is

moods

the following:

It was Saturday morning and the sun was shining.
John's
mother gave him a pail and sent him into the woods to pick
berries. The music will tell you how John felt about going.
Listen to the selection and underline the words which best

express

is

a correlation of '40

between the scores of college students on the two K-D
scores of grade-school children

The

must supply

listener

THE MEASURES OF MUSICAL TASTE
After listening to approximately 30 seconds of Victor record

number 747 11
745"8

1

(Serenade Melancolique)

(Perpetual Motion), the child states

felt glad, sorry, angry, or busy.

However,

little reliability.

The

or

seconds of

25^

whether he thinks

test

is

number

that

John

very short, and so has

for training purposes

it

may have

a real

function.

A

somewhat the same

higher-level test with

Schultz Test of Listening Power
test, it calls for

in Music.^

basic idea

Admittedly more than

the playing of phonograph discs followed by

choice questions similar to those in achievement
range from 7 ^ for junior high-school students to


Another



8

of tonal-rhythmic configurations,"

is

the

multiple-

tests. Reliabilities

for college adults.

i

along these lines, which "seeks to show.

test

3 2

is

a taste

.awareness

.

the Gaston Test of Musicalitj^^

Themes from 20 selected w^orks constitute the Gernet Music
The compositions include both "serious" music and

Preference Test.^^
jazz.

The

electrically transcribed stimuli are arran2;ed in pairs for

simple preference judgments, forming a test which has a reported
reliability oi

'

££.

Themes from 20
Musical Moods

selected

Test.'^^

The items of

seventh- and eighth-graders,

whose answers

No

little relation to

A

similar

also the

elements of the Bower

which was arranged

this test,

were prejudged by

1

7

more

sets of adjectives

reliability data are

for

music instructors,

are assumed to be the correct ones.

designate the one or

the themes.

works are

The

task

is

to

which describe each of

reported for

this test,

which has

the so-called "tonal capacity" tests,

measure

is

the Keston Music Preference

Test, in

which 120

musical excerpts, 4^ seconds in length, are arranged for judging in

groups of four. ^3 Twelve "music authorities" decided the relative

worth of each excerpt.

As was to have been expected, the twelve

rated the "serious classics" as best and the swin^ as worst.
Keston Music Recognition Test, the subject

is

asked to match

In the
a list

of

34 composers with 30 snatches of "serious" music.
161

THE MEASURES OF MUSICAL TASTE
The

Adler Music Appreciation Tests present compositions by Brahms,

Chopin, Mozart, Rameau, Ravel, and Weber, recorded on playerpiano rolls /4

Besides the original version of each piece there are

three distorted forms

The
.



a sentimental, a dull,

and

a chaotic version.

subject indicates w^hich are his most and least preferred versions,

and attempts to guess the names of the composers.

Phonograph records give the stimuli for the Mohler
Measuring Judgment of Orchestral Music.

and serious music, prejudged

as

^^

assembled into groups, whose small

more than

-^i.

The measure

is

Semeonoff, following the

has also offered sets of

preference judgment, ^^

In

were

of historic interest only, as

several of the record discs are unavailable.

Mohler procedure,

critics,

precludes a reliability of

size

now

for

Sixteen compositions of jazz

merit by 368

to

Scales

one study he

phonograph records for

also asked his subjects to

check from four possible interpretations the

mood

intended by the

composer.

An

ambitious attempt to measure musical taste has been

made by

R. B. Cattell and D. R. Saunders. ^7 Snatches of 120 compositions,
averaging 20 seconds in length, were given to 188 persons confined
to a mental hospital and to 196
data

were

factor

who were assumed

to be normal.

analyzed and eleven factors were teased out.

Unfortunately, the phonograph recording was badly done.

can be assumed that

this

subjects' judgments, and

of interest.

One

factor

structure,

But

if it

many will
have meaning, the data become

can be further granted
all

(as

was found to concern liking for popular

rhythmical

emphasis,

fast

tempo,

individual

interpretation, discordant harmonies, and joyful but agitated

A

if it

imperfection did not unduly affect the

not grant) that the emerging factors

jazz-like

The

second factor apparently involved "an attachment to

mood,

classical

music, of a sentimental, introspective but cheerful nature, with a

tendency to color harmonies,"
liking for

162

"warmth and

Another factor seemed to

stress a

gentleness," and another a preference for

THE MEASURES OF MUSICAL TASTE
"lush, romantic, fairly conventional harmonies,

nature of

The

with a flourish." The

other additional factors was not entirely clear.

still

easier of the Oregon Music Discrimination Tests (constructed

Kate Hevner and

J.

L. Landsbury) can

by

be used to learn whether or

not a subject can differentiate between short musical compositions

deemed

with

to possess merit, and versions of these compositions

distorted melody, rhythm, or harmony.

^^

The

of "meritorious" and altered compositions.

test consists of 48 pairs

Scoring credit

given

is

for recognition both of the unaltered versions and the type of

distortion to the altered version.

The

reliability of this

ranges from '47 for children of the

fifth

and sixth grades, to -63 for

measure

junior high-school groups, -78 for pupils in the senior high-school,

and '86 for

A

adults.

each of which contains

and

his

more difficult test comes in two forms,
40 items. The subject indicates his preferences

second,

degree of confidence, the latter being taken into account in

the scoring.

The

been found to

test's reliability has

borhood of '80 (college population).
odds the best of the formal auditory

fall

Both of these
tests

tests are

by

Test

for Musical

comprehension of compositions

complete wholes. ^9 While hearing a rendition of Tchaikovsky's
Symphony,

for

example,

original

as

Sixth

the listener answers a set of true-false

questions of this type: "The melody of the middle section

from the

all

of taste so far developed.

Hevner has constructed another measure, the
Concepts, to assess the subject's

in the neigh-

theme;

it is

varied,

however, and

is

is

taken

played in faster

tempo." After three hearings of the symphony, the listener checks

one statement
to do with

which run

Wing

in

each of four pairs of statements which have largely

mood.
as

Preliminary forms of this test have reliabilities

high as -Si.

has devised four tests

which have

to

do with judging the

appropriateness of the style of playing a tune.^°

recorded tune

is

In each test a

repeated either in identical or altered

notes played by the

left

hand

in the

"harmony"

test

may

style.

or

The

may not
163

THE MEASURES OF MUSICAL TASTE
be altered during the second rendition. The subject must

listen for

the possibility of a change, and then decide which of the

two

more appropriate. In the
test of "rhythmic accent" the accents may or may not fall in a
different place in the second rendition. The possibility of change and,
if there is a change, the more appropriate style of playing are to be
checked. The two other tests in this series concern loudness patterns
and phrase groups. The four tests are individually not very reliable.
But when they are taken as a battery and combined with several other
harmonizations

tests of

music

achieved.

(if

there are two)

is

the

neighborhood of -90

is

be having considerable use

in

a reliability in the

ability,

The Wing battery appears

to

Great Britain.

Lowery,"

Tests of taste have been constructed by Schoen,^^

Drake, ^3 and others. ^'^

Schoen's

Tonal

Sequence

supposedly

Test

reveals the listener's sensitivity to the fitness of the tones of a melody.

For

fitness,

Schoen suggests

balance,

criteria:

five

"belonging-

In the test, each phrase

togetherness," unity, variety, and finality.

is

followed by four terminal phrases, which must be assigned values of

Lowery's Cadence

o, 2, 4, or 6 for fitness.

In

pair of cadences heard

must be judged

Drake's

probes for the

Test of Intuition

unfinished themes. This ability

is

Test,

the second of each

more or

less

complete.

ability to supply

endings to

as

thought to be concerned with

phrase-balance, key-center, and time-balance.

The

listener judges

whether or not the second phrase of each stimulus pair makes
satisfactory ending to the first phrase.

have constructed other measures of

a

Lowery, Drake, and Schoen

abilities

which border on the

area of taste.
Practically nothing

is

known about

the degree of overlap

the areas covered by these formal, auditory

tests.

among

However, with the

stimuli often quite divergent and the test philosophies occasionally

rather dissimilar,
correlations

164

it

would be

were found

surprising

if all

to be very high, i.e., for

of the test interall

of the tests to

THE MEASURES OF MUSICAL TASTE
be measuring very similar

remembered

Moreover,

variables.

be

should

it

that the criteria of the tests for "correct" answers are

based on social judgments.

Naturally,

then,

fraction

a

of the

"correct" ansv^^ers of today will be "incorrect" tomorrow.

Paper-and-Pencil Tests

On

the theory that general musical information should be related

to appreciation, Kwalwasser has published a Test of Music Information

and Appreciation.^^

Its

arrangement

is

that of the typical school-

com-

subject achievement test, with queries about composers and

by orchestral instruments,

positions, the production of tone

The

requires

test

some 40 minutes

by M.

structed

etc.

administration.

its

Its

been found to be approximately -84 for

reliability for adults has

Stanford students.

for

A more
Young, ^^

form of

difficult

this test has

been con-

and another modification of

it

by

is

Semeonofif.^7

A

novel sort of measure, with

a reliability

of Musical Taste developed by Vernon.

around '85,

Here the subject

^^

is
is

the Test

asked to

record his reactions to 30 wholly imaginary programs of music.
the assumption that the experts

Vernon

On

has chosen to prejudge the

prograins possess taste (he has chosen as experts a panel of six
musicians),

the

score

is

defined

in

terms of the resemblances

between what the subject records and what the experts have
previously checked as the ideal.

Using

a slight

modification of the Thurstone

struction of attitude scales, Kate

developed

a Test of Attitude

subject

is

is

it

for the con-

Hevner and R, H. Seashore have

Toward Music.^9 While

strictly speaking, a test of taste,

lem. The test

method
this

measure

is

not,

bears rather closely on the prob-

composed of statements about music with which the

asked to agree or disagree.

acceptance would indicate

An example

of an item

a fairly favorable attitude

whose

toward music

is,

16^

THE MEASURES OF MUSICAL TASTE
"I

believe strongly in the beneficial and pleasurable effects of music,

but do not care to take an active part in

myself." As each test item

it

has been prejudged by a large group of people, and so

on an

may be

placed

continuum, the testee's verbal attitude can readily

attitudinal

be assessed by

this

college students

is

instrument. 3°

The

reliability as

reported for

exactly '90.

Self-rating scales have

been developed to measure interests

An

"serious" music and in several sorts of "popular" music. 3^

portant feature of these scales

in

im-

the placing of prejudged behavioral

is

characterizations along the rating lines as points of reference. Thus,
in the case of

one

"extreme

left indicates

"strongest

24 cm. in length, where a check at the extreme

scale

possible

dislike of

interest

in

characterizations are, "I listen to

make me

teachers
and,

(placed
scales,

2 1' 3

music,"

my

the

music only

listen to it" (placed 2-7

spend most of

"1

music" and one

end

at the right

two most extreme

when my

parents or

cm. from the extreme

left)

free time listening to or playing music"

cm. from the

left

end).

In an assessment of

seventh-graders and their mothers were asked

one of the
check

to

independently the musical interests of the former. The coefficient of
correlation between the ratings of the mothers and their children

was found
scale.

It

to be 'So,

indicating a fair degree of validity for the

should be added, however, that the mothers believed their

children to have

more

interest in

music than the

latter

admitted to

having.

Another, more informal measure of
list

to be assessed are given this

most eminent
the ordering

list

made by

the

becomes out of date
reflect

contemporary

is

It

of the American Musicological

should be noted that any such

course of time.

attitudes.

taste

The extent of the agreement with

members

in the

whose

with the request that they check the

ten, fifteen, or so.

Society determines the score.

166

taste involves the use of the

of 200 composers described in Chapter 6.32 Those

To be

of worth

it

list

must

THE MEASURES OF MUSICAL TASTE
The more formal tests are obviously concerned with individual
taste. With their reliabilities and validities generally mediocre or
poor, they would appear to have a rather limited future. So

seem

to have stimulated little interest or research.

next to be discussed are in no sense

what might be termed

rivals, for

far,

they

The techniques

they aim to measure

"collective" taste.

Polling
This book has already considered at

through polling, which

is

some length

data obtained

obviously not the perfect psychological

Samples polled in past studies have sometimes proved to be

tool. 33

but poor representatives of the populations with which the researcher

was

really

have

all

too often been shown to be ambiguous or impossible to

And

answer.
data.

concerned. The questions which have at times been asked

pollsters have occasionally misinterpreted their

These are among the most frequently encountered

w^here polling
are, have
taste as

is

own

difficulties

used. Yet, polling data, inaccurate as they sometimes

been found to yield roughly the same picture of contemporary

have the more operational procedures. Polling would appear

to possess, then, a degree of validity and research utility.

Orchestral Programs
Attention has been called to the fact that the Boston Symphony

Orchestra favors approximately the same composers decade after
decade.

It

is

of interest, too, that this orchestra's top choices 34

tend to be the ones also favored by the musicologists

(r/io 35

of -Si in

1944, 'jg in i95"i) and by a group of presumably typical Stanford

sophomores
the ones

(rho of '79).

who

These same composers

are,

by and

large,

have received the greatest amount of attention in the

music encyclopedias of the 1940's,

in the general encyclopedias of

167

THE MEASURES OF MUSICAL TASTE
The rho^s here run
Had the orchestral choices been
would have been even higher.

the same decade, and in the histories of music.
•82, '73, and 'jg, respectively.

limited to the 1940's, the rho's

Musicologists' and Boston Symphony's
Favorites Compared
Order of

Boston Symphony

Choice

Orchestra

0951)

Bach

Beethoven

2

Brahms

Beethoven

Bach

3

Wagner

Mozart

Brahms

4

Mozart

5

R. Strauss

Wagner
Haydn

Mozart

6

Bach

Brahms

Debussy

7

Sibehus

Palestrina

Schubert

8

Tchaikovsky

Handel

Handel

9

Debussy

Schubert

Wagner

Haydn

11

Haydn

10

Debussy

10

13

Schubert

13

Tchaikovsky

18

R. Strauss

16

Handel

20

R. Strauss

19

Tchaikovsky

66

Palestrina

30-5

Sibelius

30

Sibelius

Note:
first

Musicologists

(1944)

Beethoven

1

*

Musicologists

The names appearing below

Palestrina

the line in any one of the columns appear

among

the

nine in one of the other columns.

Unusual features of the Boston Symphony's order of favorites are
the high status of R. Strauss and Sibelius, and the low position of
Palestrina.

The idiosyncrasies of the Boston Symphony's list, however,

are relatively few.

Hence,

of taste, particularly

if

we

appear to have

at

hand

a fair

barometer

the data gathered from this organization are

pooled with those from the other leading orchestras of the land. 3^

Broadcasts of Recordings

When

the Pacific Gas and Electric Company's broadcasts (p.

i

34)

were analyzed in an effort to learn whose compositions were most
often played,

168

it

was found

that the favorites over a three-year period

THE MEASURES OF MUSICAL TASTE
coincided remarkably well with those of the Boston Symphony

Orchestra (rho of •90). Here, again, Sibelius achieved unusually high
Status.

Tchaikovsky's broadcast rank was the most markedly out of

line (using the musicologists' ballots as the

frame of reference). The

rank order of composers favored in the broadcast programs correlated
fairly

well (rho of '8^) with the eminence ranks established from the

votes of the musicologists (1944) but

somewhat

less

well (rho of 'yi)

with the eminence ranks derived from the ballots of
Stanford sophomores.

The

a

group of

hierarchies based on the relative amounts

of attention paid the musicologists' 92 composers in the histories and
in the

music and general encyclopedias of the 1940's yielded

•81, '81, and -68

from the

utility

when

of

correlated against the rank order derived

company's programs. Thus,

will give approximately the

inspection of

r/jo's

a

glimpse at the table

same picture of current

taste as will

an

some of the other barometers.

Pacific Gas and Electric Company's Broadcasts
I94I, 1942

AND

1943

Order of
Frequency

Composers

1

Beethoven

2

Tchaikovsky

3

Brahms

4

Mozart

5

Bach

6

Sibehus

7

Wagner

8

Schubert

9

Schumann

Record Listings
The composers favored

in the

programs of the Boston Symphony

Orchestra and the Pacific Gas and Electric
also to

Company

broadcasts tend

be the ones with the most recordings to their credit. Thus,
169

THE MEASURES OF MUSICAL TASTE
the rank order of the record-Usting frequencies of the early 1940's
correlates -SS with the symphony's rank order, and •91 with that of

the utility company's broadcasts.

When compared with the

eminence

ranks obtained from the histories, the music encyclopedias, and the
general encyclopedias of the 1940's, the correlation values were found
to be '87, '88, and -69, respectively.

extent of a rho of '90,

The

The musicologists agreed

to the

but the college students' value was lower, 'jg.

scores for the disc listings

distribution of recordings

were obtained by noting the

mentioned

used in counting the discs were

I.

in

each of four books. Those

Kolodin's A Guide

to

Recorded

Music (1941), D. Hall's The Record Book (1943), B. H. Haggin's Music

and The Gramophone Shop Encyclopedia of Recorded
Music (1942). The four books agree very well among themselves on
on Records (i943),37

the

number

of recordings associated with the 92 composers.

pool of any two of the book
yields

a

rho

lists

against the pool of the other

of approximately -9^.

Incidentally,

The
two

the agreement

between the 1936 and 1942 and between the 1942 and 1948 editions
of the Gramophone Shop Encyclopedia

of magnitude.

well a

number

Here, then,

we

is

roughly of the same order

have a reliable index which checks

of the other barometers of taste.

Scholarly Texts
Since histories of music and encyclopedia articles on the com-

posers are written by musicologists or their peers,

it is

to expect that the allocations of space in scholarly

works

the taste patterns of the authors' cultures.

Of course,

not too

much

will follow

the editors of

encyclopedias must usually limit the over-all space, and this necessity

must

in turn lead to a restriction of the

names can appear

in

such publications.

number of composers whose
But the relative amounts of

space devoted to the names which do pass the selection will tend to
reflect cultural attitudes.
1

70

THE MEASURES OF MUSICAL TASTE
It

must be granted, of course,

than

taste.

The

who composes

perfectionist

important composer whose life-span
space than they deserve,
character, particularly

may both

short

is

the data of

and the

receive less

the controversial and notorious

vs^hile

blessed with a long

life,

may

receive unarticle

may

own

vs^hich at

times can reach serious proportions.

many

histories

and encyclopedias are pooled, the

possess biases of his
if

relatively little

Moreover, the author of the history or

deserved space.

But

if

more

that the allocations will mirror

author-biases will tend to cancel, or at least will distort the picture
less.

Before the turn of this century,
space-allocation

eminent

men

method

in an

because no

now

be determined,

Webers and

some of the composers
Mozart

is

list.

the

first

Of

who

may be guessed

it

to aid in distinguishing

that the

may

or

in his

which of

Hence

Strausses Cattell intended.

bear these names

may not have

those whose identities cannot be questioned,

musician, with a rank of 93 in the general array

(Napoleon topped the

list

with

a rating of

i

).

of 220, Handel 261, Haydn 300, Rossini 326,

sohn 404, Palestrina 471, and Bach
appeared among the
It

employed the

There are some ambiguities

were given

initials

the several famous

in the

Cattell

attempt to discover the thousand most

Encyclopaedia Britannica was one.

been

McKeen

of history. 3^ Although the names of the encyclopedias

he used cannot

list

J.

first

475^.

Beethoven had

Wagner

No

rank

a

337, Mendels-

other composers

^00.

was to have been expected that the editors of the general

encyclopedias would allocate less space to composers than to the
military heroes and to the eminent of certain other fields.

the absolute ranks of these musicians
that these nine

tell

composers received the

us very

little.

Hence,

Yet the

fact

lion's share of attention

is

surely indicative of their relative status during the period just prior
to that of Cattell's researches.

It is

of interest that

all

with the exception of Rossini's, appear high on the

of these names,
later lists

com171

THE MEASURES OF MUSICAL TASTE
The space devoted

piled by the present author.

to this ItaUan

com-

poser has decreased slowly but steadily in both the music and general
source books written in the English language during

this century. 39

Rossini was not mentioned by a single musicologist in the 1938

survey and received few votes from the 1944 and 195^1 memberships.

Bach, on the other hand, improved in status rapidly and soon became

one of the top-ranking masters.
In the allocation studies reported in this book, the pooling of
several

works published

in

each time span has been the practice. The

general encyclopedias under scrutiny

numbered

for each period except for the 1940's, for

employed.

five

standard works

which only three were

Seven standard music encyclopedias were examined for

the period 1900-19, six for the decade of the 1920's, and five each
for the 1930's

and 1940's. Ten to twelve histories of music were

studied for each of these same periods.

Although an eminent composer

is

normally discussed in but one

section of any given encyclopedia,

number

histories

typically

mention

a

of composers on a single page and repeat mention of the

same composer on

a

number

of widely separated pages. Hence, space

measurements are usually not possible

in

work on

histories,

only practical technique so far found to be serviceable
tabulation of page-mentions.

Fortunately,

the

is

and the

that of the

number

of page-

mentions and the amount of space have been found to correlate so
highly

(r/70's

in the •90's) that for

sidered as a single technique.
It

most purposes they may be con-

4"

has already been deiTionstrated that the rankings of composers

determined on the

basis of

space-measurement and page-mention

frequencies are quite similar to the eminence rankings obtained from
the ballots of musicologists and college students, to the preference

orders taken from the programs of the Boston

Symphony Orchestra

and the Pacific Gas and Electric Company, and to the order of
frequencies secured from record listings.

172

Furthermore, the three

THE MEASURES OF MUSICAL TASTE
sorts

—the

scholarly endeavor

of

the

music

— devote rather

similar

of music,

histories

encyclopedias, and the general encyclopedias

amounts of attention to our group of 92 composers.
of the

1940's

considered,

is

histories correlates '81

When the decade

the rank order obtained from the

and -70, respectively, w^ith those constructed

from the music and the general encyclopedias. The ranks secured

from the two kinds of encyclopedias agree to the extent of
•77.

The

a rho

of

between the data of the other time periods

intercorrelations

have been found to be of approximately the same order of magnitude.

While these space
chapter are not

as

values and the related ones described in this

high as might be desired,

it

would appear

consider the space- and page-mention procedures

good picture of

taste at

safe to

as yielding a fairly

any given place and time.

Boredom
While no one
taste

has probably ever attempted seriously to measure

by testing for musical boredom, the New York Herald Tribune,

during March of 195^4, asked

its

readers to

they regarded as most boring. 4^

It

name

the ten compositions

would seem, however,

that the

New York list is unique,

that

one assembled either

Minneapolis or in San Francisco since local

in

it

should in

all

probability not resemble

conditions would be quite dissimilar in these three locales.
area

would have

its

own

particular irritations, brought

on

Each

in part

by

the too-frequent hearing of certain pieces and by the fact that other

compositions may have recently been badly played.

But one wonders

not a relatively small number

if it is

react so strongly to cliches and nuances of rendition.

Alfred Frankenstein says in the April 4,
Francisco Chronicle, there

passive followers and

is

a considerable

i9 5"4,

group of

who would

As the

issue of the San
listeners

honor what they have been taught

For them, boredom, even

if

present,

would have

critic

who

are

to honor.

little effect

on basic
173

THE MEASURES OF MUSICAL TASTE
Only the sophisticated

taste.

Of

listeners

might be seriously affected.

course, most persons tire rather rapidly of jazz pieces and other

examples of

less serious

And Rachmaninov's

music.

shelved occasionally to keep

Prelude

But such reac-

reasonably in favor.

it

tions to specific compositions have relatively little to

musical

Any school

related to taste.
it is

in favor at

against itself.

w^ith basic

is

a

it

of musical composition, no matter

is

how

any one time, sooner or later begets a reaction

Although

modified form,

Here

do

taste.

somewhat broader sense of the term, how^ever, boredom

In a

much

must be

must

it

may

later return to favor,

at least in

pass through a period of partial eclipse.

phenomenon found in all but the most static cultures, a sort
The sociologist W. I. Thomas has called this

of collective boredom.

human tendency

"the wish for

new

experience."

was

first

called to the formal tests and to

Summaij
In this chapter attention

the fact that, in general, they measure individual taste with relatively

poor

reliability

and

validity.

To

disclose the taste of classes and

interest groups, other techniques have
polling,

tions

program

among

and examina-

the relative amounts of attention paid composers in

into

scholarly

been more successful. Thus,

analyses, the counting of recordings,

works 4^ on music

all

have disclosed similar group favorites

the composers. These barometers have been found to be

internally consistent.
as it exists

They furnish ways of studying

here and elsewhere, and even

collective taste

as it existed

decades ago.

Notes
i]

Hungerland has reviewed the problem of aesthetic standards

and has offered
book.

a relativistic

view which

is

in the field of painting

consonant with that expressed throughout

this

See Helmut Hungerland, "Suggestions for Procedure in Art Criticism," 7. Aesth.,

THE MEASURES OF MUSICAL TASTE
5,

No.

A more

(1947): 189—195.

3

recent rejection of the absolutistic position can be

seen in T. Munro's "The Concept of Beauty in the Philosophy of Naturalism," Rev.
(i9SS)'- i-43-

Intern. Phil., 31
2]

The page-mention method can be considered

of the measure of space

as a variety

allocations.
3]

C. E. Seashore, Manual of Instructions and Interpretations Jor Measures oj Musical Talent,

N.Y., Columbia Graphophone, 1919.
4]

The

reliability of a test

is

degree of self-consistency, the extent to which

its

uninfluenced by factors intrinsic to or associated with
reliability

£]

6]

J.
J.

Tests

Kwalwasser and
I

low

in the high -So's or

Kwalwasser,

1930, p.
7]

one

is

induce boredom or fatigue)

as to

it

•90's.

tends to

and Measurements
P.

As

it

is

For most purposes, a reasonable

a test is

become more

in Music,

W. Dykema,

it.

lengthened (but not so

much

reliable.

Boston, Birchard, 1927, p. 26.

Manual of

Directions,

K-B

Tests,

N.Y., Fischer,

8.

The authors

realize that a lo-item test tends to have

poor

reliability

and suggest a

repetition under conditions such that the subjects believe they are hearing a 20-item test.

But even with 20 items the
8]

S.

test is quite unreliable.

A. Courtis, Courtis Standard Research

1922, p.

£.

Tests, Series

M, Music, Detroit,

S.

A. Courtis,

Before the days of formal psychological tests an even earlier attempt to study

music moods was that by B.

J.

Oilman, "Report on an Experimental Test of Musical

Expressiveness," Amer. J. Vsychol., 4 (1892): 42—83; 5 (1893): 55'8— 587.
9]

E. J. Schultz, "Testing Listening

Power

in

Music," Mus. Superv. Nat. Conf. Yearb.,

1933, 306-312.
10]

E. Gaston,

A

of Musicalitj, Manual of Directions, 2nd ed., Kansas City, Streep,

Test

1944.
11]

S.

K. Gernet, Musical Discrimination

at

Various Age

and Grade

College Place,

Levels,

Washington, College Press, 1940.
12]

L. B.

13]

M.

J.

Bower, "A Factor Analysis of Music Tests," Thesis, Catholic U., 1945.
Keston and

J. Genet. PsjchoL,

14]

M.

J.

M.

Pinto, "Possible Factors Influencing Musical Preference,"
1

01 — 113.

Adler, "Music Appreciation:

Arch. Psychol., IJ,
15]

I.

86 {13 ss):

M. R. Trabue,

An Experimental Approach

to

Its

Measurement,"

No. iio (1929): 1-102.
"Scales for Measuring

Judgment of Orchestral Music, "J.

Educ. Psychol.,

14 (1923): 545-?6i.
16]

B. Semeonoff,

"A

New

Approach to the Testing of Musical

Ability," Brit. J. Psychol.,

30 (1940): 326-340; "Further Developments in a New Approach to the Testing of
Musical Ability, with Special Reference to Groups of Secondary School Children," Brit.
J. Psychol., 31 (1940):

17]

145-161.

R. B. Cattell and D. R. Saunders, "Music Preferences and Personality Diagnosis,"

J. Soc. PsjchoL,

39 (1954): 3-24; R. B. Cattell and Jean C. Anderson, "The Measurement

THE MEASURES OF MUSICAL TASTE
of Personality and Behavior Disorders by the

18]

Music Preference Test," J. Appl.

P. A. T.

I.

37 (19^3): 446-4^4-

Psychol.,

John H. Mueller,

et al.,

"Studies in Appreciation of Art," U. Oregon Publ., 4, No. 6

(1934): 11J-130.

31-137.

19]

Ibid.,

20]

H. Wing, "Tests of Musical Ability and Appreciation,"

1

Monog. Suppl.,

Brit. J. Psychol.,

No. 27 (1948).
21]

M. Schoen,

"Tests of Musical Feeling and Musical Understanding," J. Comp. Psychol.,

5 (1925): 3i-?222]

H. Lowery, "Cadence and Phrase Tests

Music,"

in

Brit. J. Psjchol.,

IJ (1926): 11 1-

118.
23]

M. Drake, "Four New

R.

136-147; Drake Musical Aptitude
24]

The

between

difference

Tests of Musical Talent," J. Appl. Psjchol., ij (1933):
Tests, Chicago, Science Research Associates, 19^4.

a test of taste

and one of musical "capacity"

slight.

See Chapter 9 for a discussion of the latter type of measure.

2^]

J.

Kwalwasser,

26]

M. Young, "A Study of

Tests

and Measurements

and the Construction for

in Music,

is

sometimes

90—98.

the Kwalwasser Test of Music Information and Appreciation,

More

this Field of a

Reliable and

Advanced Test," Master's

Thesis, Stanford U., 1932.
27]

B. Semeonoff, op.

28]

P. E.

cit.

Vernon, "A Method for Measuring Musical Taste, "J. Appl.

Psychol.,

14 (1930):

3^5-362.
29]

John H. Mueller,

30]

The

et al.,

scale value of the

"Studies in Appreciation of Art," pp. 138-142.

example

just given

unfavorable end of the continuum and
31]

i

was found to be 4-2 where

1

1

is

the most

the most favorable.

R. Farnsworth, "Rating Scales for Musical Interests," J. Psjchol., 28 (1949):

P.

24S-253.
32]

P. R. Farnsworth,

Musical Taste,"
33]

"Agreement with the Judgments of Musicologists

J. Psjchol.,

as a

Measure of

28 (1949): 421-42^.

Q. McNemar, "Opinion- Attitude Methodology,"

Psjchol. Bull.,

43 (1946): 289-374.

34] These values were obtained through a tally of the musical items listed in the "Boston
Symphony Orchestra Programmes" for the seasons 1895-96, 1904-5 through 1907-8,

191 2-1
35]

3

through

1

91 8-1 9, and 1920-21 through 1944-45.

This rho and those which follow have as their

number

by the members of the American Musicological Society
36]

programs, "J. Abn.

Soc. Psjchol.,

"On

the

p.

(in a

123).

mathe-

Dynamic Structure of Concert-

41 (1946): 25-36.

Because of the peculiar arrangement of the Haggin book,

employ the page-mention technique.

176

1938 poll (see

For extremely interesting program analyses which are too technical

matical sense) for review here, see G. K. Zipf,

37]

of items the 92 names offered

in the

it

proved necessary to

THE MEASURES OF MUSICAL TASTE
38]

J.

McKeen

3^9-377.

Cattell,

Cattell's data

"A Statistical Study of Eminent Men," Pop. Sci. Month., 62 (1903):
were gathered many years before they were published.

Under the direction of R.

39]

ments have been taken from
different language.

2 2-

J in Mejers Lexikon.

that Rossini

Siecle, a

encyclopedias.)

When

Stempel combined

From

regarded far more favorably in

40]

It

who

(G. H. Stempel,

largest

amount of

has reported these studies, has found that

was seventh

(Note that

in space allocation.

completely out of line with what Rossini

is

placement was seventh.

countries.

was given the second

eighth place in Enciclopedia Universal (Spanish),

rank of 18 in the Encyclopaedia Britannica, and one

G. H. Stempel,

in the Encyclopedia Americana Rossini

allocation

of the University of Indiana, space measure-

of general-interest encyclopedias, each written in a

He achieved

rank of 8*^ in Larousse du XX"

of

Tangeman

worth noting

It is

space in Enciclopedia Italiana.
a

S.

five sets

is

this

given in other English-language

with those of Tangeman, Rossini's
comes the hint that Rossini may be
and Spain than in Germany and the Anglo-Saxon
his data

these data, then,
Italy

"C Sharp Minor,"

The Bloowington [Ind.] Star, August 6, 1943.)

can be taken as axiomatic that the greater the variability in the lengths of the

encyclopedia articles the
expressed.

If it is

more

exact will be the differences in eminence that can be

found that the largest encyclopedias possess

articles

with the greatest

would seem to follow that these massive treatises can supply the most
adequate material for work on eminence. Support for this possibility can be seen in the
rho of -^4 which obtains between the sizes of the encyclopedias and the spread in the
range in length

it

lengths of the articles.

41]

From most

Scheherezade,

to least boring these compositions

Missa Solemnis, Brahms's German Requiem, Dvorak's

Sjmphonj, Wagner's Tristan und
42]

were

said to be:

Cesar Franck's Symphony, Ravel's Bolero, Wagner's

Isolde,

Rimsky-Korsakov's

Parsifal,

Tchaikovsky's

Eijth

Symphony.

That the music encyclopedias make more reliable barometers of

general encyclopedias has recently been demonstrated by the author.

show
taste.

that

Beethoven's

New World Symphony, Beethoven's Ninth

taste

than the

His researches

the Encyclopaedia Britannica no longer keeps abreast of changes in musical

See "The Limitations of Cattell's Space

Method

of Studying Eminence," J. Psychol.,

44 (i9S7): 169-173-

177

CHAPTER EIGHT

The Nature of Musical

In

Abilities

the preceding chapters, the notion of capabihty has been freely

employed but
intervals,

to

in

rather loose fashion.

differentiate

To

discriminate between

major from minor, to sense

strain

or

relaxation in a melody, or even to develop a taste for a particular kind

of music, presupposes musical abilities of

some

sort.

But whether

these capabilities are largely inborn and whether only one general
ability or several

must be assumed are questions which have

not been considered.

It is

so far

time, then, that these and other important

about the functioning of our musical capabilities be

questions

examined.

Ability



In their

an Appropriate Descriptive Term

work

of describing musical capabilities, the psychological

testers have

employed

as to exact

meanings.

a variety of

To some

terms without complete agreement

authorities musical talent has

innate capability for musical performance.

Others have used the

term more broadly to include musical appreciation. Talent
latter

area

has

meant

in this

sometimes been called "musicality." The term

"capacity" also seems to bear the connotation of innate ability.
Capacities, of course, are never directly observed, but are inferred

from behavioral manifestations such
178

as

test

scores.

The term

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
"aptitude"
is

is

somewhat

the inferred ability

less controversial in that

assumed to be only in part innate.

tends to imply potentiality

It

rather than achievement, ability undeveloped before formal training

The term

has taken place.

power

"ability," suggesting the

to act but

indicating nothing about the hereditability or congenitalness of the

inferred potentiality

we

for as
jointly,

and

is

soon

shall
it is

the broadest and safest of
see,

erroneous to say that any act

either the one or the other.

all

of these terms

nature and nurture invariably function

Hence,

is

the sole result of

in the discussions of the present

chapter, conservative usage will be followed and musical ability will

be the focus of attention.

Generality of Ability
Almost everyone who has attempted
has

met persons who show

and extreme weakness

to forecast musical success

great promise along

in other areas.

Here

is

some musical

lines

who
memory

a sixth-grader

scored in the top percentile on standardized tests of tonal

and of pitch and intensity discrimination but had only chance scores

A monotone

on measures of time and rhythm.

came

to the laboratory for aid in

overcoming

to score quite well on time, intensity, and

of mature years

his disability

rhythm

who

was found

tests.

But his

pitch weakness was so complete that he could detect only a slight
difference

between the highest and lowest tones of the piano. And

the vocalist Galli-Curci, while proficient in most musical endeavors,

had such

a

poor "musical ear" that she needed an accompanist

who

could be called upon to transpose at a moment's notice. There also
are weaknesses in the affective realm, e.g., a kettle

of America's great

he abhorred

all

music

called into action.

which

early led

symphony orchestras who

It

him

in

which the

was

to

his

kettle

drummer

in

one

privately admitted that

drums were not frequently

abnormally strong interest in rhythms

work with

the percussion choir.

Cases such

179

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
would seem

as these

to yield evidence for the existence of several

rather independent musical abilities rather than a single all-em-

bracing one.
Certain of the statistically minded have attempted to answer the

question of the generality of ability through recourse to tables of
intercorrelations and to the findings of factor analysis.

out that most music tests

Hence,

indeed.

if it

now

at

They point

hand intercorrelate very poorly

can be assumed that the tests are valid measures

of musical capabilities, this evidence also

tells against

the notion of a

single musical ability.

The

more deeply

factor analysts have tried to probe

problem.
factor

into the

Unfortunately, however, the several different methods of

analysis

now

available

are

based on somewhat dissimilar

philosophies and therefore do not always lead to identical conclusions.

hardly an exaggeration to say that the English,

It is

prone to believe

in the existence of general factors,

who

are

tend to find them

in almost every set of test intercorrelations, while the Americans,
w^ith their different theories,

factors but

when he

general musical factor
tonal

memory,

general

music

find several

group

Wing

While

40 per cent of the

also

a

studied tests covering the areas of

rhythm,

pitch,

intelligence.^

tests.

factors,

more commonly

no general one. Thus, the British-trained Drake found

tonal

intensity,

scrutinizing

his

movement, and

own

English-made

found a general factor which accounted for

total variance.^

There were two additional group

one being bipolar and apparently having to do with

analysis

and synthesis, and the other concerned with harmony and melody.

Another Briton, McLeish, who gave both the Wing and the Seashore
music batteries to some
cognitive factor in each. 3

American-built
(p.

237) have

tests

little

to

Manzer and Marowitz
180

100 students,
Still

found the same general

another Briton, Vernon,

feels that the

of Seashore which stress sensory capabilities

do with music

ability. 4

to bolster this view. 5

He quotes the data of
He himself has studied

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
tests

such

163) which

the Hevner-Landsbury (p.

as

make use of

actual musical materials. These, he thinks, test a general factor,

Vernon's attitude toward the sensory
Franklin,

who

A

some

Wing,

tests

and

visual perceptual tests,

of intelligence and vocabu-

his

own music

study of the factors which emerged convinced

ability has

shared by

partly

is

has recently factor analyzed a battery of tests including

several of the Seashore, the
lary,

tests

two

aspects,

test (p. 246).

him

that

music

one being the mechanical-acoustic

(e.g.,

on

pitch, timbre, time, and intensity discrimination) and the other,
far
its

a

higher level, he terms the judicious-musical. The latter reaches
highest levels in creative musical talent.^

The most extensive study of basic music
has been that of Karlin, an American,

found eight group

factors. 7

No

abilities so far

who

The most important

manipulations.

memory

for musical passages as a whole,

A

still

factored 32 tests and

general factor emerged from the

statistical

musical elements.

undertaken

factors involved pitch,

and

recall for isolated

American study by Bower

later

also dis-

closed no huge general factor but rather three group factors.^

Bower's

first

memory,

factor

was

complex one w^hich had

a

do with tonal

pitch discrimination, melodic taste, and rhythm discrimi-

Her second concerned mood,

nation.

to

Her

nation.

third

loudness, and time discrimi-

rhythm

featured

discrimination

and

tonal

memory.
The reader
analysis

will probably agree that conclusions based

must be quite

tentative.

the tests used. Therefore,

if

there

on factor

Factors are obviously products of
is

no

test

covering some important

area of musical activity, the picture disclosed by the factor analysis
will reflect this imbalance.

of tests,

it is

For, after

economy

all,

a

When two

studies

employ

different sets

quite possible that they will report different factors.
factor analysis

is

only a

matrix of correlations.

way

of describing with

some

Since the factors depend upon

the measures used, they will be meaningful only as the tests are

181

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
With present-day music

meaningful.

tests still in a primitive state, it

follows that factor analysis can yield no definitive answers.
left,

are

and with the notion that there probably are

tional analyses can yield,

music

several poorly correlated

Ability in Music

and

abilities.

the Other Arts

Another aspect of the problem of general musical
with the hypothesis that there may exist
embraces

aptitude and

found that
Interest

taste

tests,

is

Morrow

could find no correlations of

art

measures. 9

Although Strong

score as measured by his well-known Vocational

artist

Test correlates



£j with musician score, he also noted that

the former correlates higher with a

number

of other occupational

with mathematician, -62 with dentist, -70 with

-Gi

interests:

which

ability

In an extensive study with a variety of special

between the music and

size

do

ability has to

broad

a

But here again, the experimental evidence

the arts.

all

largely opposed.

any

We

then, with whatever conclusions case studies and intercorrela-

psychologist, '79 with physician, '84 with author-journalist, and

Music interest correlates -60 with

•8£ with architect.

interests in

both psychology and the ministry.^" White, in a study of the versatility
of 300 eminent men, could find no general aesthetic type of interest
or ability."

One

small area in

average length of

life

which musicians do resemble
of their eminent fellows.

span for both musicians and

6y £
7

2*

5-

over

years. '^

7

1

,

Just

and for educators

why

artists

182

life

it

was somewhat more than

and musicians should be alike and

two occupational groups have

certainly no

the

was found to be approximately

musicians and educators different in this regard
fact that

artists is in

one study, the

That of eminent engineers, on the other hand, was

years.

slightly

artists

In

is

not clear. But the

similar longevities

justification for hypothesizing a

is

most

commonality of abilities.

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
Academic Intelligence and Musical Ability
It

has been argued that academic inteUigence

component of musical

abiHty.^3

that the musically great

average intelligence.

men

J. S.

At

is

least the fact has

an important

been established

of history possessed far better than

Bach, for example, had an estimated i.q.

somewhere between i 2^ and 140, Beethoven's was between i 35^ and
140, Haydn's between 120 and 140, Handel's between i^g and 155,
and Mozart's between 1^0 and

i^'^^.^'^

show children with high music-test

music

it

tests,

must be

when

there are studies which

scores to be significantly brighter

than their low-scoring colleagues. ^5

argument,

And

But, for the other side of the

said that scores

on the better-standardized

given to groups of limited age range,

show

little

correlation with those on tests of academic intelligence, although

the slight correlations which are found are usually positive. ^^

It

should also be noted that children of high i.q. tend to yield music
test scores appropriate to their chronological ages

but not to their

i.Q.'s.^7

Other evidence

against the notion that intelligence and musical

ability are invariably related

can be seen in data gathered on those

peculiar individuals, the idiot savants. Traditionally this term has

been applied to persons who

some well-developed

who

test

low

in intelligence but

special ability. Typical

is

who

possess

an idiot of four years

could barely articulate "papa" and "mama" but was able to sing

over ^o melodies.
earlier to

first

now

believed that

many

of the cases thought

the idiot-savant classification could better be called

fit

schizophrenics.

than had

It is

Others have been found to be

been estimated, ^^ while

intelligence, have
relative sense.

still

much

others, admittedly

been shown to be musical or

That

is,

artistic in

low

in

only a

they were imbeciles or idiots, perhaps, in

academic intelligence but morons or border-line
ability,

higher in i.q.

in

the special

not really superior. There seem, however, to be

at least a

183

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
few

who come

cases

When

savant.

Rife and Snyder addressed inquiries to

institutes for the

who

tives

ability.

close to fitting the classical picture of the idiot
5^^

American

feeble-minded they unearthed eight mental defec-

appeared to show somewhat better than average musical

^9

True

idiot savants, while small in

number, are

sufficiently

numerous

to refute the hypothesis that better-than-average musical ability

must

we

have

invariably be

And,

accompanied by high intelligence.

as

seen, the music test data indicate that within the range of school

populations, academic intelligence and the several tonal abilities have
at best
if

one

only a slight positive relationship.^" Yet the fact remains that
is

to reach the highest level of musical success

intelligence considerably above that of the average.

one needs an

^^

The Hereditability of Musical Abilities
The present-day formulation of the nature-nurture

relationship

is

not one which would have appealed to the extremists of a few
decades ago.

Whether they were

hereditarians or environmentalists,

older theorists blinded themselves to

the

the

attempts to maintain their one-sided positions.
neither nature nor nurture can alone

make

obvious in their

now

It is

a musician.

clear that

Both must be

present before musical and other abilities can emerge. The person

who

has excellent tonal and rhythmic sensitivities but

unmusical surroundings will not be

as likely to

will another with similar sensitivities

propitious

environment.

who

They are

as

meaningless

of a particular automobile
the

make of

car, or

attributable to

184

its

is

in

finds himself in a

more

Questions which ask for the relative

potencies of nature and nurture in creating
able.

who

achieve in music as

is

as

a

musician are unanswer-

how much of the speed
gasoline and how much to

questions on

due to the

what percentage of the area of

length and what percentage to

its

a rectangle

width.

is

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
more

Certain people seem to be so constructed that they react far
positively than

must be

most

of environment

fertility

But even for these cases there

to tonal stimuli.

the early interest

if

Erwin Nyiregyhazi,

turned in other directions,

a

not to be

is

prodigy

vs^ho

was

studied most carefully by Revesz, sang melodies before he could

speak and began to improvise during his third year.^^ Yet he has not

achieved the renown which might reasonably have been forecast

But Mozart,

for him.

composed

little

who

"learned" the clavier before age four and

more

pieces at age five, although perceptibly no

precocious, reached musical heights almost no one else has attained.

The

differences in the successes of Mozart and his fellow prodigy

be due,

A

at least in part, to dissimilarities in

warning should be given on the possibility of confusing musical

ability with motor skills

excellently

formed

.

History reveals youngsters whose hands were

for piano

long hours and so mastered

had parents or teachers to
and

must

environmental pressures.

how

work, who were willing to practice

many

tell

to vary their playing

difficult

them

who
and when

piano techniques, and

precisely

what

to play

from the mechanically exact. These

children admitted to no real love of music or yearning to perform or
to compose.

not their

The "performance expressiveness" of

own

their playing

but was imposed on their playing by others.

these children musical geniuses, then, or

of unique build on

whom

tonal abilities

Were

were they merely persons

optimal pressures from the surrounding

environment led to the development of remarkable motor

The more

was

^3

skills

?

appear in the child at an earlier age than

do the rhythmic. ^'^ With practice, preschool children make spectacular gains in singing tones, intervals, and phrases, but less improve-

ment

in time-keeping.

Greatly enhanced

skill in

the

last

mentioned

appears with training at a somewhat later period, as soon as better

motor coordination permits.
growth,

then,

is

Some

of the unevenness in musical

apparently due to

differences

in

maturational

readiness for the activities in question.
i«ir

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
It is

know that pitch sensitivity can be improved by
Wyatt and a number of others have shown that enormous

reassuring to

training.

changes can be produced by the use of proper training procedures, ^5
After training, Wyatt' s music-school students had

moved on

the

Seashore pitch norms
decile.

(p. 2^5) from the seventh up to the second
Her subjects who were not enrolled in a music school had

with pitch training risen from the seventh decile to the third or

growth

fourth. This

was apparent
But

is

the

was not

in sensitivity

at tonal ranges

improvement

where no

training had

the Seashore thesis

I.Q.,

were

valid,

mere

a

limits as Seashore has maintained, ^^ or

enhanced

upping; of cognitive

something more basic?

is it

one would expect children of high

with their better powers of concentration, to score higher than

more normal fellows on tonal
noted, the two groups make similar
their

scores. ^7

assume that the

It

might, of course, be added that even

effects of pitch training

we

But, as

tests.

to

Hence,

have already
it

seems

safe

on

ability are rather basic.

if

sensitivity changes

not been demonstrated so dramatically there would
to

but

been attempted.

so far demonstrated a matter of

attention and mental concentration,

If

just a coaching effect

still

had

be no reason

suppose that training methods developed later might not be

effective.

The view of nature and nurture
have led us

is,

in brief, that

interplay of heredity and
facilitates

which our considerations
is

environment.

From

these

two

facilitations abilities develop.

The organism

less

sets

limits

or

of interacting limitations and

Musical

inherited than abilities in

abilities

to a limited extent.

seem

many other

MUSICAL ABILITIES AND FAMILY LINES.

186

so far

always the resultant of the

achievement in many ways. The environment likewise aids

or inhibits.

more nor

to

an ability

in general

no

areas.

Like begcts like Only

But even where the offspring closely resemble

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
the progenitors in

causes of the resemblance.
little

impossible to determine the exact

abilities, it is

D.A.R.-like studies of family stock are of

or no value to the problem of unscrambling the roles played by

heredity and environment in the creation of musical abilities. ^^

should

come

v^ives,

only one of

as

no surprise to

whom

was musical,

resemble, on the average, their

his

two

has had

two broods of children

own mothers more

But whether the resemblance

mothers.

man

find that w^here a

It

is

than their step-

due to the biological

inheritance of ^enes transmitting musical potentiality, to a complex of

mother

fixations or

of biological
disclose. ^9
ability

is

stepmother rejections, or to some combination

and sociopsychological causes,

Whether

wholly

a

training he can be

a

person

is

wedded

cannot

the analyses

to the idea that musical

matter of inheritance or entirely due to excellent

made happy by

the same family line analyses.

If

the

genealogical research proceeds far enough back in time, the musician

who

believes in heredity can always find a musical ancestor

whom

from

may have come, while the environmentalist
absence of musical abilities among his immediate

his musical ability

can relish the

ancestors and point to

some unrelated musician or teacher

as

the

"source" of the environmental pressures which have antedated the

musical achievements.

The musical Bachs and the members of other

families of

famous

virtuosos have been carefully counted generation after generation but

with no great benefit to science. 3^ For

who

can

tell

whether the

eventual eclipse of certain of these families was due to dilution of the

musical heritage; to changes in the social, economic, and political

made other occupations more attractive to the members
of later generations; or to some combination of biological and economic forces? Genealogical research may indeed be quite necessary

milieu which

for those
light

who

crave to belong to the socially elite.

on the problem of the origin of

But

it

throws no

special abilities.

187

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
Abilities

Much

and Body Structures

of the folklore about the effects of physique on ability

outgrowth of "common sense" and primitive
folktale

is

that angular ears predispose

logic.

owner

the

is

One

3^

to

an

such

unmusical

existence since sound waves are not angular but curvilinear.

A

person fortunate to be gifted with long, thin, muscular fingers and

wide hand-span

has,

per

se,

—or

the ability to be a violinist, a pianist

Extremely even front teeth and certain textures of

a thief.

allegedly related to the ease of playing

one or more

lip

sorts of

are

wind

instruments.

So

far

no one has bothered

to

check on the relation of ear shapes

But work has been done on finger length and

to musical abilities.

slenderness, tooth evenness, and thickness of lips. 3^ Admittedly

it is

easier to play the violin or the piano if the hands are "properly"

Yet no

constructed.

between

violins, horns,

of

correlations

finger, lip, or tooth

or clarinets.

moment

have been found

measurements and

Although the

pianists

master

ability to

and

violinists of

college age so far examined do have slightly wider than average hands

and longer

younger

fingers, 33 a study of still

pianists

beginners to have shorter than average fingers. 34
dedicated musician like the ^reat violinist Ysaye,

stubby fingers, simply works harder
reach

skills

as

at his task

showed these
Apparently a

who had extremely
and may manage to

great as those his better-fingered colleagues

more

easily achieve.

The

racial

abilities. 35

.Ordinarily Nordic-lovers,

grant the Alpines,

Negroes prominence

Mediterraneans,
in

as

say

about musical

they have been willing to
Semites,

and sometimes the
arts

because

belonging to a higher order of

creativity than capability in music,
racists base

to

one or more of the nonliterary

they regard ability in literature

Most

much

determinists have had

painting,

and the other

arts.

any alleged superiority on obscure elements of

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
physique presumably caused by differences in genetic structure.

A

very few have offered sociopsychological explanations, e.g., the

musical achievements of the Negro and the
tions for their

unhappy minority

would seem wise

It

to

check on the

true, of course, that

places,

and has withered

there

if

But

is

of any particular race ?

The question would have more meaning
what constitutes

times and

other periods and in other areas.
fall

as to

"racial" group.

at certain

the blossoming correlated with the rise or

ment

before searching

some one

music has blossomed

at

are overcompensa-

facts of race

for reasons for the supposed superiority of
It is

Jew

status.

were general agree-

After decades of argument, the

a race.

physical anthropologists are in the process of discarding the term

except, perhaps, for use in separating Caucasians, Mongoloids, and

Negroids. The ancestry of most Europeans and Americans shows such
diverse strains that
cultural unities.

"X

is

that

it

To

can be described only in terms of national and
explain a person's musical abilities by saying

musical because he

is

a Slavic

Jew" can mean

X probably came from a culture area where

teachers and

where music was

especially

little

there

more

than

were excellent

honored and furnished one

of the few outlets for occupational success. 3^

As was mentioned
little

worth

earlier, research in

music testing has proved of

for "racial" assessments, 37 even for the comparisons of

whites and Negroes. 3^

In the several studies

on

racial difference,

sometimes one and sometimes the other of these two American
groups has achieved the higher mean
said

is

that whatever the

be due largely

if

mean

test score.

The most

that can be

score differences are, they appear to

not entirely to factors of the testing situations and

not to basic differences associated with

racial stock.

The explanation of sex differences in musical ability is much the same
as that for "racial" differences

difficulty

except, of course, that there

with the term. Music

the higher

mean

tests

cannot be guaranteed to

score consistently to either sex. 39

On

is

less

award

the achieve-

189

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
ment

side there

almost

all

is

prizes.

no question but that the male has so

In a

man's world

this

is

hardly to be

far

taken

wondered

at,

for the environmental pressures to succeed are largely exerted in his

direction. Such observations, however, have not dissuaded Vaerting,'*°
Schwarz,"^^ and others

creative and

is

the

bases for

from the thought

that

inherently defective in whatever
several

musical

adored

"Woman's fundamental urge
person; man's urge

as a

The statements of the
any strength.

Far

is

is

is

may be

naturally less

the biological

Moreover, the eminent

abilities.

psychologist and determined hereditarian

claimed:

woman

Carl

Seashore has de-

to be beautiful, loved, and

to provide and achieve in a career. "'^^

hereditarians are not backed by evidence of

more

must be gathered before sex

basic data

And, unfortunately, these

differences can be properly explained.

cannot be gathered until there emerges a culture in which the two
sexes have equal opportunity and equal motivation to achieve in the
arts.
It

Then and only then

will the

comparisons have real meaning.

probably would not occur to most musicians that there might be

a connection

between handedness and musical

psychiatrist

Quinan has maintained

the normal

amount of

abilities.

that musicians display
Sikes, a

sinistrality. '^3

Yet the

more than

music teacher, has

also

considered the presence of left-handedness a cue for the prognostication of later musical achievement, in this case success with the
piano. 44

However, there

Sikes's theory

affected

students.

.45

by the

is

support from piano teachers for

There was the feeling
skillful

left-hand

In piano playing the left

carry, a fact

which most beginners

ambidextrous person or one
his left

little

who

that Sikes

work

may have been unduly
more promising

of her

hand has an important load to
fail

to realize.

A more

nearly

early recognizes the inadequacies of

hand and assiduously practices

this

weaker member would have

an advantage although he was not left-handed. 4^

There

I

90

is

the implication in the writings on sinistrality that

left-

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
handedness

is

associated with mental and emotional abnormality and

that musicians have

behavior.

It

more

than their proper share of such deviant

may assume

true that an occasional musician

is

a

Hollywood-like personality which has many deviant elements, but
there

is

no reason to believe that one must have an unstable nervous

system before he can achieve in music. 47 In one study, elementaryschool children were rated by their teachers and music supervisors
for promise in music, handedness, and speech adequacy. 4^

showed

The

data

most musical possessed only the normally

that those rated as

expected number of speech troubles and amount of left-handedness.
In another, unpublished study of college students, the

most musical

and most unmusical were compared on standard personality
Here, again, there was no evidence that musical

way

tied to mental or emotional abnormality.

of Miles and Wolfe on the early

life

tests.

abilities are in

any

Moreover, the work

histories of fifty of the great

geniuses of history discloses no unusual concentration of mental or

emotional abnormality. '9

Keston has recently compared the personality profiles of students

who

score high on a music preference test of his

with others

made

quite

who

score low. 5°

similar

mean

men

ficantly higher than did their less musical fellows

scores (F, Mf, Sc, and Ma).

looked upon

men
show

with

in

America

as a relatively

feminine interest area only

slightly deviant personality patterns
It

is

But even

if

they do

musicians

as

it

enough

is

can be expected to

too early to

further researches along the line of Keston' s

deviations will be large

scored signi-

on four of the sub-

Keston speculates that with music

great interest in this art.

findings.

construction

on the Minnesota Multiphasic

scores

But his more musical

Personality Inventory.

own

His two groups of female subjects

work

know whether

will yield similar

not to be expected that the

to justify

the branding of male

psychoneurotics.

191

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
Adlerian Views on Ability
well

It is

known

that defects often spur an individual to extra-

The

ordinary achievement.

became
that

a

Demosthenes of antiquity

stuttering

famous orator. The illegitimate Smithson showed the world

though he suffered from

social inferiority

majority of his generation in
partially deaf

many

he was superior to the

And

areas of achievement.

Beethoven, perceiving that

the

was progres-

his affliction

sive,

composed

own

compositions before complete deafness could overtake him.

The

at a faster

and

faster rate in an

attempt to hear his

more dedicated

theories of at least a few of the

followers of

Alfred Adler go beyond the simple idea described above. 5^

granted that

man may be

But instead of seeing

It

is

new heights.
many wellsprings

spurred by his inferiorities to

mechanism

this

of virtuosity, these Adlerians see

as

it

as

one of the

one of few, often

as the

Jew as possessing
hearing and becoming through

major

wellspring. Thus, Rosenthal views the

a "racial

tendency" toward defective

his over-

compensation to

looked upon

And

as

the deafness

antedating

all

Needless to

Moreover,

it

is

say,

Beethoven

his
is

Had he not been

a musical giant, these extremists

Rosenthal's "proofs" are of the anecdotal

quite well demonstrated that Beethoven w^as

well on the road to musical success
difficulties.

more musical than

of the genius

signs of his musicality.

would not have become

deaf he
declare.
sort.

this sort of inferiority far

Gentile. 52

fellow

when

infection led to his hearing

53

Apparently there has so

far

been but

little

experimentation on

the subject of the Adlerian theory of musical ability. 54

In

1937

comparisons were made of the auditory acuities of two groups of
school children chosen by their teachers
the most unmusical of a group of
at

169.

The

the most musical or

acuities

were measured

seven pitch levels, for each ear alone, and for both ears together.

Of the
192

1

as either

21 comparisons, only one, that for the right ear at

1900 d.v.,

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
was what one might regard

as a really significant difference

favored the Adlerian formula, that

more

is,

the unmusical group

acuity for this pitch with the right ear.

parisons showing insignificant differences

it

some

years

later. 5^

better acuities.

At

were

this

tested at the

age the

all

other com-

same seven pitch

more musical had

fell at

The

(p.

levels

consistently

Along with the acuity tests, the students

who

showed

similar acuities. 55

given the older Seashore battery of music measures
acuities of those

this

was concluded that these

two groups of young children had very
acuities of college students

With

and

were

also

The

235").

67 percentile or above on the Seashore

were compared with the hearing scores of those who scored
33 percentile or lower.

Except

in the areas of

at

rhythm and con-

sonance the higher Seashore scorers had the better acuities.

A

similar study of junior high-school students but undertaken in a
different context

was

that

by Bower. 57 She

states:

"There

is

some

evidence here that those with superior and average hearing did better
in the tests of pitch,

rhythm, and tonal

memory

than those with

defective hearing."

The

data gathered in these studies are not incompatible with the

notion that occasionally a somewhat deaf person
in a musical direction.

(or "races"

which show

But

this

is

not to say that

more musical

musical persons

felt

auditory defects.

More

possess the better acuity.

Jungian Views on Ability
A onetime collaborator of Freud,
sively of

all

a high incidence of musical achievement) are

musical because of overcompensations to
often the

may overcompensate

C. G. Jung, has written exten-

what he has termed "archetypes." These are primordial

images, psychic residua of experiences which have happened not to
the individual but to his remote ancestors. These psychic residua act
as

unconscious forces which are basic to the appearance of musical
193
13

FSP

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
and other

artistic abilities, say

the Jungians.5^ Unfortunately for the

Jungians, the theory of the collective unconscious

out of line with

is

the thinking of all present-day biologists, except perhaps those in the

USSR,

since

involves the concept of the inheritance of acquired

it

In other words,

characteristics.

it

can be subsumed under the

now

discredited Lamarckian theory of evolution.
It

was Jung who gave the world the terms "introvert" and "extro-

vert."

with

The

his

more

introvert

own

is

attitudes

He

introspective.

tends to be preoccupied

and mental processes. The extrovert attends

to external events and objects, according to the definition.

These terms have had

a difficult history,

them

psychologists are employing

Europeans

who

think

more

and contemporary American

less

and

But the

often.

less

terms of typologies have seen the

in

several artistic abilites as closely linked to mental

musical although he

may

possess a

Here the more musical college students they

ten American composers

than were the

less

as well,

were found

picture of the extrovert.^^ They find
preferring popular to serious music.

between research

findings

measures of extroversion
other.

to be

is

at least in

tested,

still

Such

a

and

a different

him not unmusical per

se,

but

marked disagreement

when

the various

available agree so poorly

with each

not surprising at a time

now

rarely

more extroverted

Keston and Pinto paint

musical.

is

good sense of motor rhythm. 59

Gross and Seashore found quite the reverse to be true,

America. ^°

Thus,

types.

Szucharewa and Ossipowa hold that the extreme extrovert

What is really needed is a better way of describing personality.^^

Freudian Views on Ability
Music has received
sister arts,

less attention

from the Freudians than have

throughout the aesthetic

fields.

The perplexing problem of the

Freudian symbol was considered in Chapter

194

its

but the basic psychoanalytic assumptions seem similar

£.

The other elements of

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
Freud's system are equally difficult to handle. ^3

Little

is

described in

an operational form which would permit of ready experimental
verification. Explanations in

terms of instincts tend to be tautological,

and Freud's explanations seem to be no exception. Moreover, such
concepts

as

sublimation are slippery. ^4 Xhe Freudians, and everyone

but in explaining

else for that matter, can see sex in the cancan,

dance form there
sex

is

is

no need for the concept of sublimation. Where

not obvious,

basically sexual

the only "proof" that the energy source

comes from the process of psychoanalyzing.

unfortunately, the psychoanalytic interview

mind of

analyst later takes out as his proof.

course, adds up to no proof at

present stage,

may have

all.

the analysand

Such a process, of

So, w^hile psychoanalysis, at

therapeutic utility,

it

is

And,

in great part at least,

is,

a process of indoctrination, a putting into the

what the

this

its

has not as yet provided

a consistent set of scientifically verified explanations for the origin of

the several artistic abilities.
largely

on

It

must,

if it is

to be accepted,

be taken

faith.

Imagery as a Source of Abilities
Sir Francis Galton,

one of the

to

first scientists

work with mental

imagery, thought he had discovered pure image types.

But

later

researches convinced the psychological fraternity that most persons
all

sense fields, with the

most

vivid in the auditory.

have images of considerable strength in
strongest in the visual area and the next

Musicians, of course, tend to have

more

intense auditory images than

do the unmusical and may be above average
thetic areas as well. ^5

Von Weber,

in the tactual

for example,

was

extraordinarily strong visual and auditory imagery.

One
that

a

and kines-

musician with

^^

of the most extensive comparative studies of imagery has been

by Agnew

who

has carefully rated the "mind's ears" of

many

run-of-the-mill musicians, psychologists, and children, ^7 as well as

13-2

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
of great composers. ^^

She has developed an imagery questionnaire

which has enjoyed some

The German

use.

scientists, particularly,

have noted the existence of

imagery of hallucinatory intensity, w^hich they have termed "photographic" or "eidetic."

almost

Imaginal material appears to the eidetiker

normal perception. Virtually unbelievable

as in

told of the abilities of eideiikers

who, by reading

a

tales

have been

book or

a score

only once, or by listening to one rendition of a symphony, could then

without obvious cues reproduce the material
rehearing. ^9
tikers,

and so presumably

Cowell.7°
Sistine

as

if

rereading or

Mozart, Gounod, and Berlioz were undoubtedly eideis

the contemporary composer

Henry

Mozart's famous "theft" of the Miserere after visiting the

Chapel only twice was accomplished through the aid of his

eidetic imagery.

The

early appearance of strong auditory imagery in the child

may

serve as a predisposing factor to subsequent ability with, and interest
in,

tonal materials.

The evidence

so far collected indicates that

images can be cultivated and that the absence of a functional sense
organ, e.g., as in complete deafness,

imagery in that sensory area.

more common among

is

always paralleled by absence of

Eidetic images are

children than

among

adults,

known to be far
many of the latter

having lost their eidetic potentialities through lack of practice.

Developing Abilities
It is

possible for the organism to respond to sudden, loud noises

even thirty days before birth.

where the

Several instances have been observed

foetus has jerked convulsively

were sounded

close to

the mother.

when

tones of high intensity

Unless there

is

anatomical

impairment, the child normally shows considerable sensitivity to
tone shortly after birth, and by the eighth day he will usually stop
feeding at the sound of a gong.7i

196

Quite naturally, the small infant's

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
depend

reactions to tone will

condition of the moment,

The

first

e.g.,

to

some extent on

two-note cadences sung by the very young child tend to

be descending fourths and major thirds,

Werner

his physiological

whether or not he is sleepy or hungry. 7^

according

to

Piatt. 73

agrees that the early cadences are the descending ones but

minor third appears

feels that the

first. 74

The octave

is less

frequently

attempted and the ascending and other descending cadences are tried

As the child matures and

less often.

sions and chord figures, he learns the

than he does the

age employ

them

mean

presented with scale progres-

former with

far greater facility

latter. 75

When singing voluntarily,
for

is

children four and a half to eight years of

pitch levels significantly lower than those arranged

in their

song books; the mean of their voluntary pitch

range, approximately

9*5"

semitones,

is

smaller than that

of them by their printed songs, which average about
But, without

much

strain,

young children can,

cover a considerably greater tonal span,

as

i

if

o*

demanded

7^
g semitones.

they really try,

Froschels has shown. 77

His four-year-olds had a range of eight semitones, his five-year-olds
ten semitones, his six-year-olds eleven, his seven-year-olds fourteen,

and

his

eight-year-olds sixteen semitones.

found even higher values

—age

Jersild

and Bienstock

four, thirteen semitones; age five,

seventeen; ages six and seven, twenty-two; and age eight, twentyfour semitones. 7^ These researchers report that with

some practice

there can be expected at least a 30 per cent gain in the

number

of

tones three-year-olds can sing. 79

Outstanding musical
age seven.

In fact

Brown ^^ would

abilities are often

lead one to suspect that by age seven the typical child

has matured to the point

begun.

noted considerably before

the studies of Garrison, ^° Cochran, ^^ and of

Unusual

where piano

lessons

may be

profitably

ability in painting usually appears at a later age,

presumably because the motor

skills

necessary for handling art tools

are not sufficiently developed until after the seventh year.

197

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
The

show

a

decided preference for the traditional concordances, and then only

if

must be nearly nine before he

typical child

will

he has been subjected to some Western subculture. ^3 But
old he needs to be before he will get the

minor dichotomy
delights

is

not clear. Walker,

complicated typologies,

in

full

who

has

just

how

import of the major-

like

many

drawn

Continentals

most involved

a

picture of the growth of the modal discriminatory powers. ^4
data at least

make

it

clear that the child only gradually develops a feel

Particular trouble

for these affective associations.

minor, which
pleasant.

His

seems merely dull and perhaps

at first

Only much

comes with the

later does

it

slightly un-

begin to take on a clearly sad

affect.

That the growing child steadily improves his discriminatory powers
in the several tonal areas

C. E. Seashore found

music

In his earlier

it

is

shown by the

fact that

music

testers like

necessary to offer several sets of age norms.

test battery.

Seashore presented separate norms

for the fifth grade, for the eighth grade, and for adults. ^5 His current

battery offers one set of norms for the fourth and
for the sixth, seventh, and eighth grades, and

nine through sixteen.

fifth

still

grades, another

another for grades

^^

Attention has already been called to the peculiarly thin tone of the
preadolescent male soprano, a tone with

The female appears

to pick

up

this

less

than normal vibrato.

ornamentation

much

earlier than

the male, possibly because she matures faster than he does. ^7 In both
sexes there

is

a

change in tonal quality and

a

widening of the pitch

range at the time of puberty, slight in the female and quite marked in
the male.

It

goes almost without saying that puberty in the male

period of considerable musical
less

strain.

Not only

is

his voice

a

under

firm control as he shifts from a higher to a lower register but his

status has changed.

He

has

begun to assume an adult role and will

from now on be compared with other
suffer

from

this shift in

adults.

Prodigies particularly

frame of reference and many such exceptional

children leave the musical spotlight shortly after this period.

198

is

For

it

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
is

one thing to be compared with other child performers but quite

something

be rated on

else to

continuum along with

a

a Heifetz or a

Kreisler.

Training Methods: General Problems
Musical learning might be expected to follow in general the rules
of

all

learning.

Questions regarding whole versus part learning,

motivation, overlearning, prestudy and mental rehearsal, distributed
versus massed practice, beta learning, and retroactive inhibition, are

encountered here

as

In addition, there are other pro-

elsewhere.

blems met solely within the music

Under what

conditions,

sections

example,

for

is

whole

more

learning

Should one go over the material

efficient than part learning?

whole, time after time, or

area.

it

is

wiser to break

with practice restricted pretty

much

it

up into smaller

to

these

smaller

portions ? Research on music materials checks rather well the
in other learning areas.

Where

as a

work

the material to be learned seems very

long to the memorizer so that he tends to become discouraged and
lose morale, the part

method

is

superior. ^9 But

where the

learner's

method and he is not overawed by the length of the score he must learn, the whole method
wins out. 90 The student's aim, then, should be to work with as large
prior habits are not too tied to the part

a portion of his score as

makes

persons this means that

as

a

manageable unit for him. For most

learning proceeds, longer and longer

scores can be treated as a single unit.

The

literature reveals but

one experimental study

in the musical

area on motivated versus unmotivated music learning, and this one

would appear

to be quite limited in applicability.

In each section of

the Rubin- Rabson study one of three different sorts of incentives was
operative. 9 1

In the first of the experimental situations the only

incentive was what the learning process itself provided.

No

verbal

199

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
encouragement or other goad was employed.

In a second, there

were many exhortations from the experimenter.
there was promise

of

money payment

if

And

in a third,

improvement became

Rubin-Rabson's data revealed no differences among

especially good.

number of trials needed to bring
agreed upon level of achievement.

the three stimulus situations in the
the
It

skill

up

to a previously

should be noted that the Rubin-Rabson study does not prove that

learning efficiency will be the same irrespective of the type of
incentive.

What

does demonstrate

it

is

that rather forceful incentive

changes must obtain before the slope of the learning curve will be

much affected, more forceful than any that Rubin-Rabson employed.
One is reminded of an adult monotone who was being taught to
His improvement had been unmistakable and

discriminate pitches.

remained exasperatingly slow until

was

quite steady, but

it

knocked from

head during the singing of the national anthem. At

his

his hat

this

point his learning curve swept sharply upwards and maintained

for

some time much of

its

new

standing can undoubtedly recall

slope.

Any music

somewhat

teacher of long

similar instances

her pupils where spurts in learning speed occurred
"proper" incentives were

To

as

among

soon

as

come upon.

learn an act, say the educational psychologists, practice should

be continued beyond the

trial

where the material can

for the first

time be reproduced correctly. The material, in other words, must be
overlearned. Valid as this principle

seems to be

in

most

hold in the extensive musical studies of Rubin-Rabson,

her subjects to practice

5^0,

it

did not

who

forced

areas,

100, and even 200 per cent

more

than

was necessary for bare learning. 9^ Nothing was measurably gained by
all this

added

effort.

notion that while
favorably sheer

it

Rubin-Rabson explains her
is

200

with the

conceivable that overlearning might affect

motor performance on the piano,

affect the learning of piano

a

finding;

it

music since the activity here

matter of meaning and insight.

should not so
is

much more

Consequently, once memorization

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
is

achieved,

"needs only to be restored to

it

original clarity

its

on

subsequent occasions. "93 With no other studies to contradict those

her conclusions must be

of Rubin-Rabson,

at

least

tentatively

accepted.

Although Kovacs,

as early as 1915',

attempted to ascertain whether

keyboard practice actually takes

careful inspection of a score before

place might not benefit the subsequent learning of the score, his

experimental controls were so poor that no generalizations could
safely

be drawn. 94 Hence,

Rubin-Rabson

rely again

upon the researches of

no other psychologist or educator has worked

The questions Rubin-Rabson attempted

this area. 95

in brief, (i)

(2) if

as

we must

mental prestudj of benefit to subsequent learning, and

is

will mental rehearsal

it is,

in

to answer w^ere,

be of value

learning process? Affirmative answers

at

other periods in the

were found

to

both questions.

The best period for mental rehearsal was found to be a time roughly
midway among the keyboard practice sessions. Thus, it would appear
that the ambitious piano student should not only analyze and study
his scores before

he

starts his

formal keyboard practice but should

take off time considerably before his top

skill is

reached to rehearse

mentally what has been going on.

The work of Rubin-Rabson must once

on the problem of massed versus

generalizations

With

again be

all

to

one continuous session or to

several sessions?

For the learning of

educational psychologists favor

some

split

for

distributed practice.^^

limited time to spend on learning something,

allocate

employed

is

it

wiser to

the effort between

but the easiest material,

all

sort of distribution or spreading

of the practice

trials,

and Rubin-Rabson' s data on the learning of

piano music

in line

with their generalization. She

fall

of one hour betw^een

trials

vals

far

more than two time

must be experimented with before

allocation

is

an interval

group of students and

for her first

24 hours for the second. Admittedly,

set

best for any given situation.

it

can be

said just

inter-

what

But with some labor,

a

201

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
music Student can,
sort of material

Common

he

if

will, find the

optimum time spread

for each

he desires to master.

sense warns against the practicing of one's mistakes.

Repeated again and again, these errors will grow into habits and

become

But

eradicate.

difficult to

is

this belief in the

power of

practice really sound?

Would

almost

a matter not of avoiding but of repeating

all

practice

mistakes ? For

The

if

is

it

not be more accurate to say that

an act can be done perfectly,

fact that practice

is

why

skills

at all

it

?

being

made

as associated

with

needed shows that errors are

even though the learner may not recognize them
the individual

practice
still

he wishes to acquire. In ordinary learning, then,

there is practice of errors along with a rehearsal of the correct elements

Why not force the practicer to become acutely aware of his errors?
asks the psychologist Dunlap.97 Why not have the learner single out
his mistakes

and rehearse them alone but do so with the ever-present

desire that they can and should be

Dunlap

calls beta learning in

eliminated? This procedure

contradistinction to the

more

ordinary

form, where the learner does not restrict his practice to his errors
but

drills

himself on a medley

mistakes in the

hope

While McGeoch and

made up

of both correct acts and

that the latter will gradually

be eliminated.

Irion categorically state that beta learning has

proved effective "in correcting errors
studies to support their

contention. 9^

in piano-playing," they cite

no

Moreover, the only published

report of the use of the beta technique in the entire music area, that

by Wakeham on the elimination of errors
only of

its failure. 99

It

might be added that the present writer has

twice tried beta practice on musicians
persistent performance errors and
instances.

carry out

Perhaps

some

Wakeham and

detail of

performance errors

in

who were bothered by

was quite unsuccessful

in

both

the author unwittingly failed to

Dunlap 's methodology.

Or

it

may be

that

music are somehow unlike typing errors where

the scheme has proved so successful.

202

in organ-playing, tells us

But, in any event, there

is

no

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
reason

as yet for

replacing the

more ordinary procedures with

the

beta variety.

There

is

the distinct possibihty that the learning process

thrown badly out of gear should the learner practice two
or

less

Evidence from

concomitantly.

that this possibility

becomes

The

tasks are quite similar.

number

practically a certainty

learning of the one

learning of the other task, a
It

a

phenomenon

of areas suggests

whenever the two

somehow

To

may prove

inhibits the

called retroactive inhibition.

behooves the musical educator, then, to ascertain

this principle

how

important

to be for keyboard learning.

obtain at least a partial answer, Rubin-Rabson observed the

behaviors of i8 highly trained musicians in a

number

of situations.

Happily, no important inhibitory effects were detectable
tasks

may be
more

tasks

when two

were learned concomitantly. To quote Rubin-Rabson:
[My] conclusions are not unexpected. The experimental pro-

cedure was only a repetition of

had long since

many

a learning situation familiar to

The mechanics of piano study
accustomed them to learning much new music

these subjects for

concomitantly while
various degrees.

No

years.

retaining

material

already

learned

to

confusion develops transferable from one

learning to the other because the organizational skill of these
learners
bits of

is

highly trained and specific and because rarely are

two

music so similar in key, rhythm, melodic or structural

details as to

make

involuntary transfer feasible. There

is

here,

furthermore, a favorable task-set engendered by the prestige
factors implicit for musicians in a music-learning situation. ^°°
It

would be

a bit

premature to suggest that the Rubin-Rabson

research has completely settled the problem. While
will

most probably be found

generally, they
at this
final

may

or

may not

lower proficiency

its

conclusions

to hold for well trained musicians

apply to beginners.

level, then,

Further research

must be undertaken before

a

answer can be given.
203

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
Training Methods: Special Problems
While
will, to

has long been

it

some

known

that practice with the right

hand

extent, also train the left hand, this fact has certainly

led no one to limit his practice to his right hand. Yet certain music
teachers have believed that practice

would

the other

with one hand and then with

first

result in quicker learning than drill

with the two

hands in coordination. The notion seemed to be that while one hand

was resting

it

would be absorbing more

practicing than
rested with
are at least

the

it.

two

if it

unilateral.

^^^

continued to practice with

However, the

facts

studies available

superiority

The

from the hand that was

skill

of the

do not support

mate and then

this idea.

There

which demonstrate beyond question

coordinated

student, then,

its

is

technique

as

opposed to the

advised to attempt hand coordina-

tion from the very beginning of practice.

Proper imagery

is

most important

needed for "good" voice quality

is

for

particularly difficult to achieve but,

according to Bartholomew, training in

The

and "image" of

"feel"

resonance

at 5^00

this area

tonal quality represented by strong

violinist,

vocalist.

particularly

Good kinesthetic imagery is
when he begins to practice

double stops. ^°3 At

this stage in his learning,

longer

guide

as effective a

a single string.

can be effective. ^°^

and 2800 cycles are essential for the male singer and

around 3200 for the female

needed by the

a

music training. The imagery

For in

as

it

auditory imagery

is

no

was when he was learning to play on

his earlier practice

on the

single string

he

could by careful listening almost instantly adjust his finger position

whenever
it

it

becomes

rely

was incorrect. But with two

difficult to

know which

fingers breaking the strings

finger to change.

on kinesthetic images which refer

So

now he must

specifically to a single finger.

Too much emphasis, however, must not be placed on imagery.
Indeed, direct sensory cues are often more essential. Thus, beginners
on the piano learn more slowly if they are kept from looking at their
204

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
hands and the keyboard than

if

allowed to look where they will/"'^

This finding follows the basic learning principle which states that,

other things being equal, the

more

the sensory cues available the

faster the learning/^5

Several excellent studies

on

sight reading are available to

the reader.

Although space here allows for but few comments, the serious
student will find an examination of the original articles cited in the
references of the next

fev^^

pages most w^orthwhile.

The exposure of

musical material on cards offers one approach to the study of sight
reading, and photography of eye fixations furnishes another excellent

source of data.

moving hands and the

In certain of the studies, the

hammers within

the piano have been photographed.

Bean warns piano teachers to allow their pupils to gain reading
speed in the early stages of learning even
errors.

^°^

He

finds all too

to single notes
that

is,

when

many persons

at the

expense of occasional

reading slowly and attending

they should be attending to musical patterns,

to short phrases.

Reading individual notes

to individual letters while reading, and this

is

is

akin to attention

behavior typical of

extremely poor readers. By the judicious use of flash cards the reading
of most students can, Bean says, be speeded up appreciably and be made

considerably

more

who knows what

accurate.

Ortmann

points out that the teacher

her pupils' eyes can and cannot do will be better

equipped to suggest proper training methods. ^°7

As

a result of their researches,

Lannert and Ullman believe that the

piano student should be early taught to read ahead of the measure

being played and should be forced into considerable sight-reading
practice. ^^^

known

The arrangement of

that little visual attention

the keyboard

need be paid

must become

it.

so well

Lowery also stresses

the great need for sight-reading practice and the early achievement of

smooth eye-hand coordinations. ^°9 He notes
far

more complicated than

that reading

music

is

reading print, since musical symbols are

not arranged on lines or columns but are scattered both horizontally

20^

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
and

vertically.

between

The eye often

staves.

on

fixates

Music reading

areas

between notes or even

made even more

is

difficult

of our unscientifically arranged staff and symbols.

because

The work of

Wheelwright, "° for example, clearly shows that the spaces between
notes and rests should, for purposes of better reading, be proportional to the represented time values.

A

wealth of material on the reading and playing of music can be

found

in

the excellent reports of Weaver,"^ of

Weaver, ^^^ and of

While these

Jacobsen.-^^^

Van Nuys and

studies

were not

primarily oriented toward training procedures, they do offer helpful
suggestions for the

improvement of

will be given here, however, since

extended coverage of

that

all

Only

practice.

this

book

is

a

few samples

not the place for an

might be found helpful to the music

teacher.

The reading of music should

start

with chromatics and accidental
perceive.

much

in the bass clef

which are very

and that written on leger

difficult to

lines cause

reading difficulty which can only be overcome by extensive

practice.

often

Music

with diatonic intervals and not

signs,

Surprising as

more needed by

it

may seem,

practice on reading

words

is

the beginning music student than practice on

note reading. Immature students should be introduced rather early
to scale runs and only

much

later to arpeggios.

Emphasis should be

on speed reading rather than on accuracy since the habit of slow
reading

is

difficult to break.

reader, not the slow one,
his advice

who

On
is

with the pessimistic

material for beginners

is

the average,

more
comment
the

it

is

accurate.

the

fast

music

Jacobsen ends

that inuch note-reading

not well adapted to their reading level and

should be replaced by scores better geared to their perceptual
capacities.

206

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
Creativity

No
as

one

really understands the intricacies of the creative processes

they function in any particular composer, not even the composer

himself. This fact, however, has not stopped several musicians

from

introspecting and retrospecting on these interesting processes.

For

example, the modern composer Henry
psychologist-mentor, L.

how

M. Terman,

Co well,

stimulated by his

has described in

and for a time having

little

detail

access to musical instruments, Cowell

diligently practiced imagining the timbres

finally getting

imagined

in the traditional

effects

manner.

he had been imagining.
string

massage,

many

different timbres,

not offered by any instrument played

Later,

Cowell, he experimented with

He was
imagery. With

he had heard.

aided in this labor by the possession of eidetic
practice, he put together in his "mind's ear"

his

some

he believes he composes. "4 Early deciding to be a composer

it

when

to elicit

In this fashion

a piano

some

was available to

of the bizarre effects

were born

his

tone clusters,

and the other Cow^ellian timbre

novelties.

Whenever Cowell was commissioned to compose, he employed
effects which seemed to him appropriate to the occasion, realizing,
however, that he was offering

his

listener

no clear-cut musical

message.

Research which many composers might not consider entirely
realistic

was that performed by Benham when,

in the interests of an

experiment, he composed a series of nine-measure melodies (average

time between 60 and 70 seconds). -^^5
strongest

at

the

He found

emergence of each musical

his auditory

idea.

imagery

During the

development there were motor sensations and other types of
imagery. The major danger in this and other similar experiments, of
course,

is

that the overly analytic mental set, necessitated

experiment,

may have

by the

interfered with the experimenter's creative

powers or have given them rather

different qualitative flavor.

207

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
Benham were made by
well-known European com-

Observations somewhat similar to those of
Bahle,

who

sent questionnaires to 32

posers in an effort to learn
replies

by

a

were

later

number of
In

centuries.

how

they believe they composed. ^^^ Their

checked against autobiographical documents

left

the greatest European composers of the past three

one of

his

many

studies

on the problem of

creativity,

Bahle asked his composer respondents to set poems to music and to
introspect and retrospect on the process.

Unfortunately his data,

extensive as they are, have led to few generalizations which could

made before

not have been

problem was

respects Bahle' s

the reasons for their long
that are aired

—yet

life.

were undertaken.

In

some

like that of asking centenarians

about

the studies

Many and

varied are the convictions

no one knows with certainty the degree of

one of the theories offered to explain either the

validity of any

longevity of an individual or his creativity.

By comparing better and poorer students of composition, Gross
and Seashore have helped validate the commonly held belief that
composition comes in part

facility in

at least froin toil

and sweat. ^^7

Formal and informal training and knowledge of good work habits
appeared in

this

study as extremely important for

compose. Whittaker and
reflect

the

creations

particular

were shown

folk art of the

the

life

who would

found compositions to

composer's informal

training. ^^^

Musical

to be generally in line with the traditional and

immediate culture and the rather narrow interests of

community

The

his associates also

all

in

which the composer

sociologists Lastrucci"9

lived.

and Becker "° have studied the way of

of dance-band musicians and the effects on creativity and per-

formance which stem from their extremely atypical living habits Dance
.

musicians feel forced to compose and perform in idioms and manners
appreciated by their audiences. Such pressures from the "ignorant" lay
public they often resent and compensate for by striving to produce,
at least in

208

jam

sessions,

what they regard

as

higher level material.

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
It

is

popular nowadays to point to a reified unconscious

major wellspring of

Max

logist

Graf

artistic inspirations.

Thus, the great musico-

asserts that the greater part of musical

takes place in the unconscious mind,
entity that science cannot

as the

which

to

him

is

formation
a mystical

measure or explain/^^ Graf does admit,

however, that the composer's childhood memories and

his life-long

environmental pressures affect his style of composition.
Jancke makes the point that most

not

creativity

is

by psychological tensions which are often unconscious.

^^^

if

all

preceded
In

some

degree the act of composing relieves these tensions. To the com-

more than to the listener, music is autistic and
personal. Hence, when the composer listens to one of his own
earlier compositions, it may take on a quite different meaning from
poser, then, even

what

it

originally

what

is

perhaps a rather different tension.

To make

his

had for him, since

own

unconscious

deliberately fatigued himself and

at this

at least

time

it

serves to lessen

somewhat recordable, Loar

went without

his

normal

sleep. ^^3

Then he attended concerts where he drowsed. When Loar
attempted to record

his

dreams and reveries he found that no

repressed wish or inhibited desire could be recognized
Instead, there appeared in fictionalized

fantasies.

which Loar had formed from
times.

However,

his reading of the

in fairness to analytic theory,

that Loar's technique

later

among

composer's
it

his

form memories
life

and

must be admitted

was not one which could uncover repressed

wishes.

Why

some composers

beautiful"

is

create "beautiful" music and others "un-

explained by Ehrenzweig on a Gestalt-psychoanalytic

basis. ^^4 Aesthetically

good

Gestalts, i.e., beautiful tonal materials,

belong to the surface layers of the mind. The "depth mind" or unconscious, on the other hand,

is

"Gestalt free."

Hence

it is

from the

unconscious that the poor Gestalts, the ugly and the distorted, come.

Although the modern composer's unpleasant music may seem highly
209
14

FSP

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
sophisticated, his art, thinks Ehrenzweig, represents a retrogression
to the least differentiated

modes of

The composer may honestly
to

infantile "thing perception."

believe his compositions to be novel,

be born from an unconscious that has had

or no

little

the music of his contemporaries or of the past.

But the

with

traffic

fact

that

is

the composer forgets the origins of much of the material he will later

come from

use in his creations. This material has

and

is

unconscious in the sense that

As time goes on
later

it

will

a variety of sources

not immediately recallable.

it is

be elaborated into the form

be produced. But just

how much

goes on below the verbal threshold

which

will

it

of the rearrangement of items

not known.

is

amount must often be considerable

in

as

However, the

Haydn

certain composers,

and Schumann, for instance, seem to have done their creative work
without

much

effort.

But others, like

J. S.

and erased and followed extremely rigid
to be a continuum, then, along
"intuitive" at the

Bach, typically scratched

rules.

There would appear

which composers

one end and the "non-intuitive"

fall

—with

at the other,

the

with

the former yielding his finished product almost without considered

thought and the latter only after

much

careful deliberation and

conscious elaboration.
Biographical studies of the great composers usually stress their
personality structures, their psychological abnormalities and sociological uniquenesses, as well as the cultural forces

been

in part responsible

embraced and

both for the

for the fact that they chose

Luck, special body build, extreme
standingly

style of

relatives or friends, high intelligence,
facility, vivid

imagery, abnormally fine

as a career. ^^5

and joy of
health,

for

2IO

living,

pressures

out-

from

extreme tonal and rhythmic

memory,

past suffering,

greater than average ambition and persistence, ability to

work on

to have

composition they have

composing

vitality

good or extraordinarily poor

which seem

make

much
others

one's behalf, poorly controlled emotions, inherited talent

composing,"^ readiness to forgo present pleasures for future

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
and even willingness to be a melody thief have

gains,

mentioned by one or another biographer
Biographical research

genius.

is

necessary for creative

as

fascinating w^ork,

pay big dividends in the years to come, yet

at

conclude that some one of

his

which may indeed

present

too crude to allow for valid generalizations.
likely to

A

many

its

methods are

biographer

and emphasize

this item.

It

too

is all

biographical items has

great causal significance simply because his biases encourage
select out

been

all

him

to

can be expected, perhaps, that

over the years biographer biases will in some degree cancel each

But until that happier day arrives, only hunches and very

other.

tentative conclusions can be safely

material.
It

is

drawn from

this

type of research

^^7

when neurolowas found by Lehman to be

interesting to note that the period of

muscular coordinations are

at their best

life

when eminent composers of the past were at the peak of
creativity. ^^^ Of course, temporal coincidence does not neces-

the time
their

sarily indicate causality.

maximum
so

many

creativity

areas



and philosophy,

Yet the correspondence between decade of

and years of best motor coordination

well as musical composition

considerable significance.

motor

skill

as

simply do not have

as

much

is

to

Wayne Dennis

somehow

men,

Hence, the date of

work

maximum

available time.

seems to find that topflight workers

Lehman and Ingerham

that

related, asks us not to

keep producing to an advanced age with surprisingly
In a study by

may have

it

particularly those in academic

some extent determined by

^3^

that

free time to devote to creative

do their younger brethren. ^^9

creativity



However, Bjorksten, while agreeing

and creativity must be

forget that middle-aged and older
life,

mathematics,

athletics, painting, writing, science,
as

exists in

who

live

And
long

little letup.

of the compositions of eminent

deceased musicians, the half-decade from age thirty-five to thirtynine was shown to be the most productive for grand operas, cantatas,

and orchestral and symphonic works of "superior" quality. ^3i
21

14-2

I

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
This research team continued

its

study with contemporary American

composers, but apparently met with great difficuhy in deciding what
compositions should be placed in the "superior" category.
this uncertainty

makes

less

meaningful the

creative half-decade for this group

from
in

fifty

to fifty-four.

The peak

both studies to come
It

for

fell

later

peak



in the years

music of high quality was found

earlier than that for sheer quantity.

inust be admitted that the above discussions of creativity leave

us with a deplorable lack of closure.

answers sought in

this area

sometimes proved to be

menon

It is

have been

quite possible that the

bound

is

they have

as unrealistic as

in certain other areas.

Rarely

of nature found to have a single cause.

question
is

much

Perhaps

that the

finding;

If

is

a

pheno-

the process in

broad in scope and appears in many different contexts,

to be related to a host of variables.

We now

know,

it

for

example, that the several sorts of leadership demand different psychological qualities, although for years researchers sought a single set of

psychological characteristics that

would be

typical of

doubt the creative processes, too, show multiple
blossoming of creativity

is

all.

Without

causality.

^3^

The

surely dependent in considerable part

on

circumstances unique to particular situations. The trigger which
actually sets off the creative process in

similar

from

that

one

man may

needed for another, particularly

different personality or school of composition,

be quite
if

he

is

dis-

of a

or from another

culture or age.

Summarj
The layman

often speaks of his friends as extremely musical, as

moderately musical, or perhaps
that there

is

as

not

at all musical.

one general musical or even broader

one may possess to

a greater or lesser degree.

implies

art ability

which

The evidence, however,

points to a contrary conclusion, to the existence of a

2X2

He

number of

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
semi-independent musical

music, just
a

most other

as in

no one of Avhich seems particu-

abilities

other

larly related to abilities in the

While top achievement

arts.

in

academic intelligence of

areas, calls for

high order, the correlation in the school years between music test

scores and i.q.

who

is

slight.

much

few very stupid persons have been

In fact a

less stupid in

the musical realm. These are the

In the main, spectacular musical

achievement seems unrelated to

located

are

idiot-savants.

anomalies of gross body musculature. This should not be taken to

mean

that there are not neural constitutions admirably suited to

musical endeavor.

Yet these constitutions are largely wasted

proper environmental pressures are not operative.

aware of the constitutional
musical accomplishment.
prodigies

who

who

misfits

But

we

are

all

if

well

struggle in vain toward

are less apt to note the

numerous

sooner or later drop out of the musical picture because

of the infertility of their musical surroundings.

modern

We

It

is

the view of

science that the course of development of neither group can

be predicted from scrutinies of their family

weaving of nature and nurture

is

far

Indeed, the inter-

lines.

too complicated for the successful

use of such a simple device as genealogical analysis. Achievement and

no means identical, and the fact that one man has
more in music than another, or men in general more than
women, or members of one national or racial group more than
potentiality are by

achieved

another cannot be transcribed into capacity terms.

A

wide range of

achievement can be derived from similar potentialities.

To

describe a person as "arty"

psychoneurotic.

who

It is

is,

in

some

circles, to

as

true, of course, that history reveals musicians

have been neurotic or even psychotic.

between musicality and abnormality,
slight.

brand him

if

there

But the connection
is

one,

is

extremely

Standard personality tests and ratings disclose no relationship

between the two.

A

possible exception to this generalization con-

cerns the jazz performer,

whose way of

life

typically encourages

213

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
unstable relations with his family and other deviant behaviors as
well.
In this chapter, the claims of Adler, Jung, and

scrutinized for their bearing

on musical

abilities.

Freud have been
It

was concluded

that while auditory insufficiencies can facilitate musical creativity,

such weaknesses are not essential to composition and performance of
high order, even though the Adlerian extremists would have us
Jung's view that abilities arise as psychic residua of

think so.

ancestral experiences

was dismissed

Lamarckian notion of the inheri-

logical belief, being a variant of the

And

tance of acquired characters.

out of line with current bio-

as

the extraversion concept was held

to be too poorly defined for purposes of quantification.

notion of sublimation was also regarded

What

as

of

little

The Freudian

explanatory aid.

are needed are operationally sound hypotheses susceptible to

eventual verification.

Sublimation seems not to be a concept of this

sort but rather something to

While

be accepted

as

an act of

musicians have auditory imagery of

all

faith.

more than average

strength, a few, the eidetikers, possess auditory pictures of halluci-

natory intensity.

Such imaginal

enter a musical career.

He might

possession of eidetic imagery

By and

skill

is

could well encourage a child to

enter anyway, however, for the

not

a sine

large, the rules of learning as

qua non of musicality.

formulated by the educational

psychologists are found to hold for the learning of musical materials.

But each

field of learning has its

exception.
learning as

unique problems, and music

Overlearning, for example,
it

has been found to be in

because the activities here are

more

a

than of sheer motor performance.
facilitated

by proper increases

slighter goadings

214

is

not

as beneficial to

many other

areas,

is

no

music

presumably

matter of insight and meaning

Music learning

is

of course

in motivation although not

by the

found of value in many other sorts of learning. Beta

learning, the intentional practice

on

the elimination of typing errors,

fails

errors,

which works so well

with musical materials.

in

And,

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
at least

with well-trained musicians, retroactive inhibition does not

appear to be the hazard

in

it is

some

fields

of learning.

Students of musical creativity are gradually giving up the idea that

come from

the abilities of the composer

authorities have so far gotten little

common

a

beyond the

stage of enumerating

up

possible essentials to creativity and of playing
favorites.

The more mystically minded

creativity

at least a

is

is

now

are techniques
all

now

and

consider

problem

Gradually, however,
later yield a clearer

Introspection, retrospec-

available.

treatment of age data

statistical

our concern with precocity and creativity

number

those

as special

—here

being used.

forget that every musician
ficiency in a

few

the receptacle from

few data are being assembled which may

tion, biographical analysis,

With

as

alleged to spring full blown.

picture of creativity than

a

theorists sidestep the

with their positing of the unconscious mind

which

wellspring and

Unfortunately, however, most

are independent of the social setting.

must reach

of basic abilities.

capabilities

at least a

In

we

our next chapter

assumed to be needed,

measurement has long been one of the

should not

minimum

of pro-

we

since

shall

their

tasks of the psychological

aesthetician.

Notes
i]

R.

M. Drake,

"Factorial Analysis of

Technique," J. Musical.,
that his early conclusions
to musical ability

l

were

—-music

Music Tests by the Spearman Tetrad-difference

(1939): 6-16.
in error.

Over the

He now

memory and rhythmic

years,

Drake has convinced himself

two important

facets

See Drake Music Aptitude

Tests,

feels that there are
ability.

Chicago, Science Research Associates, 19^4.
2]

H. D. Wing, "A Factorial Study of Musical Tests,"

3]

J.

McLeish

U. Press,

Brit. J. Psjchol.,

in The Fourth Mental Measurements Yearbook,

New

31 (1941): 34i-3i'S'.

Brunswick, N.J., Rutgers

i95'3.

4]

P.

Vernon, The

£\

C.

W. Manzer

Structure of

and

S.

Human

Abilities,

N.Y., Wiley, 19^0,

p. 93.

Marowitz, "The Performance of a Group of College Students on

the Kwalwasser-Dykema Music Tests," J. Appl. Psychol., 1^ (i93f): 331—346.

21^:

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
6]

Franklin,

E.

Gumperts
7]

J.

Tonality As a Basis for the Study of Musical

"A

Karlin,

E.

Talent,

Goteborg, Sweden,

Forlag, 195^6.
Factorial Study of Auditory Function," Psjchometrika, 7

2^1-279; "Factor Analysis in the Field of Music,"

J. Musicol., 3

8]

L. B.

Bower, "A Factor Analysis of Music Tests," Thesis, Catholic U.,

9]

R.

Morrow, "An

S.

Mechanical Abilities,"
10]

K. Strong,

E.

Analysis of the Relations

J. Psychol., 5

1945'.

Tests of Musical, Artistic, and

(1938): 2^3-263.

Manual Jor Vocational

Jr.,

Among

(1942):

(1941): 41-5^2.

Blank Jor Men,

Interest

Stanford,

Calif.,

Stanford U. Press, 194^'.
11]

R. K. White, "The Versatility of Genius," J.

12]

C. Alexander, "The Longevity of Scientists," J. Soc. Psychol., sp (19^4): 299—302.

13]

M. Schoen,

14]

C. Cox, Genetic Studies of Genius, vol.

i^]

C. F. Lehman,

the
16]

W.

(1931): 460-489.

The Psychology of Music, N.Y., Ronald Press, 1940.
2,

Stanford, Calif., Stanford U. Press, 1926.

"A Study of Musically Superior and

Kwalwasser-Dykema Music
R.

Soc. Psychol., 2

Tests," J. Educ. Res.,

Inferior Subjects as Selected by

4s (19^2): 517—5^22.

Lundin, "The Development and Validation of a Set of Musical Ability Tests,"

Psychol. Monog., 63,

No. 10 (1949).

For a resume of the studies in

this area see J. L.

Mursell, "Intelligence and Musicality," Education, 59 (1939): SS9~S^^17]

L. S. Hollingworth,

J. Educ. Psychol.,
18]

"The Musical

Ehrsam, "Uber den

E.

Sensitivity of Children

Who

Test Above

i

3^ i.Q.,"

IJ (1926): g^^- 109.
Fall einer einseitigen

Kindes bei hochgradigen Leistungsriickstand,"

musikalischen Begabung eines blinden
Neurol. Med. Psychol., Leipzig,

Psychiat.

7

(19^^): 149-1^419]

D. C. Rife and

S4-7~SS9-

^^^ ^ls°

L.

in Human Inheritance," Hum. Biol., 3 (193 i):
Owens and W. Grimm, "A Note Regarding Exceptional

H. Snyder, "Studies

^-

^-

Musical Ability in a Low-grade Imbecile,"
20]

Of

average.

J. Educ. Psychol.,

32 (1941): 636-637.

from high schools sometimes show music students to be brighter than

data

I.Q.

course this finding could conceivably

mean

that

music students per

se

have

higher than average i.Q.'s.

More

and that

not the rule (D. K. Antrim, "Do Musical Talents Have Higher

this superiority is

Intelligence?"
21]

Etude, 63 (194^):

The claim

ability

is

has

been made

plausible

is

the hypothesis that the surveys

were

selective

127-128).

that

it is

not academic intelligence per se to which musical

related but rather mathematical ability.

To check on

this persistent belief,

G. Revesz has surveyed both mathematicians and musicians and has found no unduly large

number

of musical mathematicians or mathematically

the Psychology

oj Music,

Norman, U. of Okl.

Press,

minded musicians

{Introduction to

135^; "Beziehung zwischen mathe-

matischer und musikalischer Begabung," Schweiz. Z. Psychol. Anwend., 5 (1946): 269—281).
22]

G. Revesz, The Psychology of a Musical Prodigy, N.Y., Harcourt Brace,

1925-.

See also

C. Stumpf, "Akustische Versuche mit Pepito Areola," Zsch.f. Ang. Psychol., 2 (1909):

i-ii;

F.

Baumgarten, "Der Werdegang eines Wunderkindes," Zsch.f. Ang.

(1932): 473—498.

216

It

is

Psychol.,

41

of interest that there have been virtually no child prodigies in

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
'cello, clarinet, flute,

conductors.

or voice, and but few in composition. The style

most

In the past the

fertile areas for prodigies

N. Slonimsky, "Musical Children, Prodigies or Monsters?"
23]

R. Updegraff, L. Heiliger, and

J.

were

Etude,

now

violin

is

for prodigy

and piano. See

66 (1948): 591-592.

Learned, "The Effect of Training upon the Singing

Ability and Musical Interest of Three-, Four-, and Five-year-old Children," U. of la. Stud.

Child Welf., 14 (1938): 83-131 A. T. Jersild and S. Bienstock, "A Study of the Development of Children's Ability to Sing," J. Educ. Psychol., 2S (1934): 481—503; M. S. Hattwick
and H. M. Williams, "The Measurement of Musical Development, II," U. of la. Stud.
Child Welf., 11 (1935): i-ioo; G. E. Moorhead and D. Pond, "Music of Young Children,"
Pillsburj Found. Stud., 1941, 1942; E. N. Drexler, "A Study of the Development of the
Ability to Carry a Melody at the Preschool Level," Child Bevel., 9 (1938): 319-332.
;

24]

H. Christianson, Bodily Khjthmic Movements of Young Children in Relation to Khythm in
Educ, No. 736, N.Y., Teachers College, Columbia U.,

Music, Teach. Coll. Contrib.

1938; A. T. Jersild and

S.

Bienstock, "Development of

Rhythm

in

Young Children,"

Child Devel. Monog., 22 (1935).
25]

R. F. Wyatt, "Improvability of Pitch Discrimination," Psychol. Monog., ^8, No.

2

(1945); A. A. Capurso, "The Effect of an Associative Technique in Teaching Pitch and
Appl. Psychol., 18 (1934): 811-818; E. Connette, "The
Knowledge of Results," J. Educ. PsjchoL, 32 (1941): 523—532;
H. Pyle, "An Experiment in Individual Training in Pitch-Deficient

Interval Discrimination," J.

Effect of Practice with

M. Wolner and W.

Children," J. Educ. Psychol., 24 (1933): 602-608; G.

M. Whipple,

"Studies in Pitch

Discrimination," Amer. J. Psychol., 14 (1903): 289—309; R. H. Seashore, "Improvability
of Pitch Discrimination," Psychol. Bull., 32 (1935): 546. For training data on other music
abilities see

Psychol.,

G. M. Gilbert, "Sex Differences in Musical Aptitude and Training," J. Gen.

26 (1942): 19-33.

26]

C. E. Seashore, Psychology of Music, N.Y., McGraw-Hill, 1938, p. 57.

27]

See L.

135 i.Q."
28] H.

S.

Hollingworth, "The Musical Sensitivity of Children

J. Educ. Psychol.,

Koch and

F.

Who

Test above

ly (1926): 95—109.

Mjon, "Die Erblichkeit der Musikalitat," Zsch.J.

Psychol., 121

104—136; H. Stanton, "The Inheritance of Specific Musical Capacities,"

(193 i):

Psychol. Monog.,

31 (1922): 157—204; V. Haecker and T. Ziehen, "Beitrag zur Lehre von der Vererbung

u.s.w.," Zsch.J. Psychol., 1931, 121, i— 103; R. S. Friend, "Influences of Heredity and

Musical Environment on the Scores of Kindergarten Children on the Seashore Measures of
Musical Ability," J. Appl.
N.Y., Stokes, 1939.
29]

J.

Psjchol.,

23 (1939): 347-357; A. Scheinfeld, You and Hereditj,

Mjon, "Zur Erbanalyse der musikalischen Begabung,"

Hereditas,

7 (1926): 109-

128; G. Voss, "Die Familie G.," Dtsch. Zsch.J. Nervenhk., 83 (1925): 249-263.
30]

C. Terry, The Origin oj the Family oj Bach Musicians, London, Oxford U. Press, 1929;

K. Geiringer, The Bach Family, N.Y., Oxford U. Press, 1954.
31]
at

In the category of folktales

is

the belief that music ability arises through being

an astrologically propitious time

(p.

118).

There

is

one

folktale

which

bom

says that

musicians do not enjoy normal longevity and another which states that they live beyond

217

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
their "proper" span. These notions, however, have
statisticians,

who

find the Uves of musicians to

been disproved by the insurance

be of normal length. See

W.

Schweisheimer,

"Do Musicians

Live Longer Than Others?" Etude, 6j (1949): 54.- ss; A. H. Whittaker,
"Occupational Diseases of Musicians" in Music and Medicine, N.Y., Schuman, 1948. Note
(p. 182),

however, that musicians enjoy

less

musicians but the difference

Some

is

not

Differences

Physiological

whose

longevity than educators,

above average. The blood pressure of musicians

is

slightly

life-span

is

lower than that of non-

statistically significant (L. F.

Sunderman, "A Study of

between Musicians and Non-Musicians:

L

Blood-

pressure," J. Soc. Psychol., 23 (1946): 20^-2 1^).
32] C. J.

Lamp and N.

Keys, "Can Aptitude for Specific Musical Instruments be Pre-

dicted?" J. Educ. Psychol., 26 (1935^): S^l—S3^33]

J.

H. Taylor, "The Relation between Finger Length, Hand Width and Musical

Ability," 7. vlppi. Psychol.,
34]

S.

Graf,

20 (1936): 347-3i'2.

"Measurements of Hand Length, Muscular Control, and Motility Related to

Handedness," Master's Thesis, Syracuse U., 195^2.
35]

E.

Bauer, E. Fischer, and F. Lenz,

Human

Heredity,

N.Y., Macmillan, 193

1;

R.

Breithaupt, "Pianistic Talent and Race," Etude, 42 (1924): 4^5-4^6; E. Kretschmer, The
Psjchology of
is

that of S.

Men oj

Genius,

Giinther.

N.Y., Harcourt, Brace, 193

Western music,

says he,

is

i.

A

typical racist generalization

better integrated than that of the

Dinaric people but lacks the empathic or emotional potential of the latter ("Rassenseelen-

kundliche Beitrage zur musikalischen Stilforschung," Arch. Musikjorsch

.

,

3 (1938):-

^8g-

427). E. Rittershaus maintains that most creative musicians of the nineteenth century had

Nordic features ("Die Vererbung musikalischer Eigenschaften," Arch.

Rass.- u. Ges.-BioL,

29 (193^): 132-1^2).
36]

R. Braine, "The Making of a Virtuoso Violinist," Etude, 43 (192^): 1^7-158.

Sward, "Jewish Musicality in America,"
that in

Keith

IJ (1933): 675^-712, points out
1932 one-half of American violin virtuosos, maestros, and first violinists of
J. Appl. Psychol.,

symphony orchestras were of Jewish descent. Ten per cent of American composers were
also Jewish.

Yet ten- and eleven-year-old Jewish and gentile youngsters score similarly

and intensity discrimination, tonal movement, and tonal memory. It
would appear, then, that the presence of so many Jews in American musical life must be
due to economic and social factors rather than genetic causes.

on

37]

tests of pitch

G. B. Johnson, "Musical Talent of the American Negro," Mus. Superv.

J.,

is (1928):

81, 83, 96.
38] The study of D. Van Alstyne and E. Osborne, "Rhythm Responses of Negro and
White Children Two to Six," Monog. Soc. Res. Child Devel., 2 (1937): 4, is almost the only
research where Negro superiority has seemingly been found. Negro children appeared to
be markedly better in motor rhythm, the superiority being greatest for the simplest
rhythms and the youngest subjects. But whether these data are to be explained on genetic
grounds, are due to errors of selection, or may be attributed to the Negroes being less
inhibited or receiving more musical encouragement is not clear. Other studies, e.g., that
of R. L. Streep, "A Comparison of White and Negro Children in Rhythm and Con-

218

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
sonance," J. Appl. Psychol., IS (1931): ^3— 7I) have found slight Negro superiorities but
never differences of such impressive magnitude.
39]

G. M. Gilbert, "Sex Differences in Musical Aptitude and Training," J. Gen.

Psychol.,

26 (1942): 19-3340]

M.

Vaerting, "Die xnusikalische Veranlagung des Weibes," Zsch.J. Psjchother. Med.

Psychol.,

41]

y (191 y): 120—127.

H. D.

Schv^^arz,

"Die Kunst

als

seelische Kraftquelle

fiir

die Frau," Psychol. Rundschau,

3 (1931): ^2-^342]

C. Seashore,

43]

C. Quinan,

of Beauty in Music, N.Y., Ronald Press, 1947, p. 367.

In Search

"A Study of

Sinistrality

workers and Others," Arch. Neur. and

and Muscle Coordination in Musicians, Iron-

Psjchiat.,

J (1922): 3^2; "The Principal

Sinistral

Types," Arch. Neur. and Psjchiat., 24 (1930): 3^-47.
44]

M.

45]

P. R. Farnsworth, "Musical Talent and the Left hand," Sch. Mus., 32 (1932): 11.

46]

For data which picture the ambidextrous

J.

L. Sikes, "Musical Talent

Kwalwasser, Exploring

47]

M.

E.

the Musical

East, "Insanity

J. P. Foley, Jr.,

J. Gen. Psychol.,

and the Left hand," Ped. Sem., 30 (1923):

than average in tongue-agility see

Mind, N.Y., Coleman-Ross, i9SSy P- 132.

and Genius,"

"A Survey

as better

56-161.

1

J. Hered.,

of the Literature

2S (1941): 111-142;

on

2^ (1938): 2y£-^j9; A. Anastasi and
Abnormal, I,"

Artistic Behavior in the

"II," Annals,

N.Y. Acad. Sc, 42 (1941): 1-112;

Monog., S^ (1940):

1-71; "IV," J. Gen. Psychol., 2S (1941): 187-237.
P. E. Vernon believes that composers of the romantic school tend to have been more
Psjchol.

"III,"

neurotic than those of the classical school ("The Personality of the Composer," Music and
11 (1930): 38-48).

Letters,

48]

P.

R. Farnsworth, "Ratings in Music, Art, and Abnormality in the First Four

Grades," J. Psjchol., 6 (1938): 89-94.
49]

C. C. Miles and L.

S.

Wolfe, "Childhood Physical and Mental Health Records of

Historical Geniuses," Psychol. Monog.,

47 (1936): 390-400.

A

study by R. V. Burton

("Are Musicians Normal?" Overture, 3S, No. £ (195s)' ^°) on approximately one-third
of all musicians employed in the motion picture studios of the Los Angeles area revealed

no unusual amount of emotional abnormality. The

Zimmerman Temperament
50]

M.

J.

Keston,

testing device

was the Guilford-

Survey.

"An Experimental

Investigation of the Relationship

between the

Factors of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory and Musical Sophistication,"

Amer. Psjchol., 11 (i95'6): 434.
5-1]

A. Adler, "Character and Talent," Harpers, 1S5 (1927): 64-72.

£2]

H. Rosenthal, "Die Musikalitat der Juden,"

I

22—1

53]

Int.

Zsch. J. Indiv.-psychol., 9 (193 1):

3 I.

P.

C. Squires, "The Problem of Beethoven's Deafness," J. Abn. Soc. Psychol., 32

(1937): 11-62.
54]

P. R. Farnsworth, "Auditory Acuity and Musical Ability in the First Four Grades,"

J. Psychol.,

6 (1938): 95-98.

219

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
5^]

weakness among artistic and
what might have been expected from

In a parallel study S. Atwell looked for evidence of color

Her

inartistic children.

findings

the Adlerian doctrine in that

it

were contrary

was the

artistic

to

youngster

who

tended to possess slightly

better color vision ("Color Vision in Relation to Artistic Ability," J. Psychol., 8 (1939):

P.

56]

Psychol,

R. Farnsworth, "Further Data on the Adlerian Theory of Artistry," J. Gen.

24 (1941): 447-4 i^oBower, "A Factor Analysis of Music Tests," Thesis, Catholic U.,

57]

L. B.

58]

M. Bodkin, "Archetypal

1945^.

Patterns in Tragic Poetry," Brit. J. Psychol., 21

(1930):

183-202.
59]

G. Szucharewa and

S.

Ossipowa, "Materialen zur Erforschung der Korrelationen

zwischen den Typen der Begabung

u. d. Konstitution," Zsch. ges. Neurol, u. Psjchiat.,

100

(1926): 489-^26.
60]

B.

Gross and R. H. Seashore, "Psychological Characteristics of Student and Profes-

sional Musical

61]

M.

J.

Composers, "y. Appl.

Keston and

J. Genet. Psychol.,

62]

A number

I.

M.

Psychol.,

25 (1941): 1^9-170.

Pinto, "Possible Factors Influencing Musical Preference,"

86 (195^): loi — 113.

of researchers have attempted to study personality variables through the

construction of a tonal equivalent of the famous Rorschach Ink-Blot
elicit

imagery and attitudinal

sets

which are interpreted more or

Test.

Music

is

used to

less in the fashion

of the

O. Grimmett, "Personality Diagnosis
through Music," Master's Thesis, Stanford U., 195^0. Personality has also been studied by
Rorschach. For one of the studies in this area, see

J.

the aid of musical preference tests.
63] S. Freud,

A General

Introduction to Psychoanalysis,

N.Y., Liveright,

H. Racker, "Contributions to Psychoanalysis of Music," Awer. Imago,

1935'.

^^^ ^l^o

8 (195^1):

129-163.

For a discussion of the Rankian adaptation of Freudianism see A. Michel, Psychoanalyse de
la Musique, Paris,

U. de

Paris, 19J1.

Michel connects the oral stage of sex development

with the use of the piano, the anal with the trumpet, and the phallic with the

flute.

64] For an attempt to link Mozart's creativity with sublimation see A. H. Esman,
"Mozart, a Study of Genius," Psjchoanal. Quart., 20 (195-1): 603-612.
65]

A. Hartmann, "Untersuchungen uber metrisches Verhalten in musikalischen Inter-

pretationsvarianten," Arch. Ges. PsjchoL, 84 (1932): 103—193; K. L. Bean,
Visual, Auditory,

and Kinesthetic Imagery

in the

Piano Keyboard," y. Educ. Psychol., 30 (1939): 5^33-^41;
Factors in the Recall of Musical Experience,"
66]

P. C. Squires,

"The Use of

Transfer of Musical Notation to the
J-

Brit. J. Psychol.,

Mainwaring, "Kinaesthetic
23 (1932): 284-307.

"The Creative Psychology of Carl Maria von Weber," Char, and

Pers.,

6 (1938): 203-217.
67]

M. Agnew, "A Comparison

of Auditory Images of Musicians, Psychologists, and

Children," Psychol. Monog., 31 (1922): 268-278.
68]

M. Agnew, "The Auditory Images

279-287.

220

of Great Composers," Psjchol. Monog., 37(1922):

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
69]

R. Kochmann,"Uber musikalische Gedachtnisbilder,"Z5c/i.J~. Ang.

Psychol.,

23 (1924):

329-35-1; T. Zaworski, "Akustyczne wyobrazenia ejdetyczne," Kwart. psychol., 13 (1947):

156-203.
70]

H. Cowell, "The Process of Musical Creation,"

3J (1926): 233-236.

ylmer. J. Psychol.,

Forbes and H. B. Forbes, "Fetal Sense Reaction: Hearing," J. Comp. Psychol.,
7 (1927): 353—355; K. Fleischer, "Untersuchungen zur Entwicklung der Innerohrfunktion," Z. Laryngol., 34 (1955): 733-740.
71]

H.

72]

M. W.

S.

Haller,

Pure Tones, "y.
73]

W.

Pets.,

74]

Piatt,

"The Reactions of

Genet. Psychol.,

Infants to

Changes in the Intensity and Pitch of

40 (1932): 162-180.

"Temperament and Disposition Revealed

in

Young Children,"

Char, and

2 (1934): 246-251.

H. Werner, "Die melodische Erfindung im friihen Kindesalter,"

Klasse

Phil. -Hist.

182 (19 17); T. F. Vance and M. Grandprey, "The Evaluation of the
Musical Capacity of Nursery School Children," Proc. la. Acad. Sci., 36 (1929): 321-328.
Sitzungsberichte,

For further data on the music of preschool children see M. G. Colby, "Instrumental

Reproduction of Melody by Preschool Children," J.
75]

Composed

M.

Bull.,

30 (1933): 21-23; D- Doig, "Creative Music: Music

for a Given Test," J. Educ. Res.,

S.

3S (1941): 263-275; 35 (1942): 344-355.

Hattwick, "The Role of Pitch Level and Pitch Range in the Singing of Pre-

school, First-grade, and Second-grade Children," Child Bevel.,
also
in

H. M. Williams, "Immediate and Delayed

Tonal Sequences," U. of

77]

47 (1935): 413—430.

A. Wells, "A Comparison of Chord Figures and Scale Progressions in Early School

Music Learning," Peabodj

76]

Genet. Psychol.,

E. Froschels,

la. Stud.

"Untersuchungen

Memory

4 (1933): 281-291. See

of Preschool Children for Pitch

Child Welf., 11 (1935): 85-94.
iiber die

Kinderstimme,"

Zentralbl.

f.

Physiol.,

34

(1920): 477-484.
78]

A. T. Jersild and

F.

S.

Bienstock,

"A Study

of the

Development of Children's

Ability to Sing," J. Educ. Psychol., 2^ (1934): 481-503.
79]

A. T. Jersild and

S. F.

Bienstock, "The Influence of Training on the Vocal Ability of

Three-year-old Children," Child Bevel., 2 (1931): 272-290.
80]

K. C. Garrison, "Psychology of Special Abilities," PeatoJ;'

Re/?ector,

12 (1939):

1

1-13.

See also R. Leibold, "Kind und Metronom," Zsch. Pddag. Psychol., 3J (1936): 317-322;
M. Varro, "The Musical Receptivity of the Child and the Adolescent," Mus. Teach. Nat.
Ass.

Proc, 1943, 77-88.

81]

M. Cochran,

82]

R.

"Kinesthesis and the Piano," Austral. J. Psjchol., 8 (1930): 205-209.

W. Brown, "The

Relation between Age (Chronological and Mental) and Rate of

Piano Learning," J. Appl. Psjchol., 20 (1936): 511-516.
83]

C.

W.

Valentine, "The Aesthetic Appreciation of Musical Intervals

Children, and Adults,"
84]

Brit. J. Psychol.,

among School

6 (19 13): 190-216.

E. Walker, Bas musikalische Erlehnis und seine EntwicMung, Gottingen,

Vanderhoeck

u.

Ruprecht, 1927.

221

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
8j]

C. E. Seashore, Manual of Instructions and Interpretations of Measures of Afusical Talent,

N.Y., Columbia Graphophone, 19 19.
86]

C. E. Seashore, D. Lewis, and

J.

G. Saetveit, Manual, Seashore Measures of Musical

Talents,

N.Y., Psychol. Corp., 1956.
87]

For a genetic study of the voices of 44 choir boys see

F. J. Hell, "Physiologische

und

musikalische Untersuchungen iiber die Singstimme der Kinder," Arch. Ges. Thonel., 2

(1938): 6^—102. Another study which compares the musical productions of children from

by A. Nestele, "Die musikalische Produktion im Kindes-

preschool to adolescence

is

alter," Beihefte z. Zsch.
f.

Ang. Tsychol.,

that

1

£,

No. ^2 (1930). For

M. Van

musical abilities of the adolescent see

still

another study of the

Briessen, Die Entwicklung der Musikalitdt in

den Reifejahren, Langensalza, Beyer, 1929.
88]

The

reader

material of this section

who would know how

learning

is

referred to the text by

Silver Burdett,

195^1.

Music,PaTtl,"

199—213.

Brit. J.

Still

work of

largely limited to the

J. L.

psychologists.

The

Mursell, Music and the Classroom Teacher, Boston,

which show the

Articles

psychologist are those by

is

the professional educator regards the problems of music

insights of

both the educator and the

Mainwaring, "Psychological Factors in the Teaching of

J.

Educ. Psychol., 21 (19^1): loj-i

another publication worth reading

is

2

i

;

that

"Part

Applied Musicianship,"

II.

by M.

Wilson, How

E.

to

Help

Your Child with Music, N.Y., Schuman, 19^1.
89]

C. C. O'Brien, "Part and

Tsychol.,

90]

R.

Whole Methods

in the

Memorization of Music,"

W.

Brown, "A

Comparison of the

Methods of Learning Piano Music,"

'Whole,'

y. Exp. Psychol.,

11

and

'Part,'

L.

E.

Whole and
Eberly,

Approach,"

Part

"Part versus

Columbia U., 192
91]

the

J.

Whole Method

Educ.
in

Psjchol.,

Memorizing

'Combination'

235-247; G. Rubin-

(1928):

Rabson, "Studies in the Psychology of Memorizing Piano Music,
of the

J. Educ.

34 (1943): jf2-^6o.

31

III:

A Comparison

(1940):

Piano

460-476;

Music,"

Thesis,

i

G. Rubin-Rabson, "Studies

in the

Psychology of Memorizing Piano Music, IV: The

Effect of Incentive," J. Educ. Psjchol., 32 (1941): 45-54.

92]

G. Rubin-Rabson, "Studies

A Comparison

in

the Psychology of Memorizing Piano Music,

VI:

Two Forms

of Mental Rehearsal and Keyboard Overlearning," J. Educ.
Psychol., 32 (1941): 593-602; "VII: A Comparison of Three Degrees of Overlearning,"
of

J. Educ. Psjchol.,

93]

32 (1941): 688-696.

Rubin-Rabson,

G.

"Mental

and

Keyboard Overlearning

in

Memorizing Piano

Music," y. Musical., 3 (1941): 33-40.
94]
1

I

95]

S.

Kovacs, "Untersuchungen iiber das musikalische Gedachtnis," Zsch. J. Ang. Psjchol.,

(1916): 113-135.

G. Rubin-Rabson, "The Influence of Analytic Prestudy

Arch. Psychol., 31,

Piano Music, V:

32 (1941

222

):

A

loi-i

I

in

Memorizing Piano Music,"

No. 220 (1937): 1-5^3; "Studies in the Psychology of Memorizing
Comparison of Prestudy Periods of Varied Lengths," J. Educ. Psjchol.,
2.

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
G. Rubin-Rabson, "Studies in the Psychology of Memorizing Piano Music,

96]

Comparison of Massed and Distributed Practice,"
97]

K. Dunlap, Habits: Their Making and Unmaking, N.Y., Liveright, 1932.

98]

J.

McGeoch and

A.

II:

A

270-284.

y. Educ. Psychol., 31 (1940):

A. L. Irion, The Psychology oj Human Learning, N.Y., Longmans

Green, 1952.
99] G.

Wakeham, "Query on 'A

Science,

Law

Revision of the Fundamental

of Habit Formation',"

68 (1928): 13 i'— 136.

G. Rubin-Rabson, "Studies in the Psychology of Memorizing Piano Music,

100]

The

Same and of Different Degrees of Learning," J.

Inhibitory Influence of the

VIII:

Musical., 5

(1947): ^S-

W. Brown, "The

R.

loi]

Two Methods

Relation between

of Learning Piano Music,"

16 (1933): 43^-441; G. Rubin-Rabson, "Studies in the Psychology of
Memorizing Piano Music, I: A Comparison of the Unilateral and Coordinated Approaches,"

J. Exp. Psychol.,

J. Educ. Psychol.,

W.

102]

30 (1939): 321-34^.

T. Bartholomew, "Imagery in Voice Pedagogy," Peahodj Bull., 31

20—28; "The Paradox of Voice Teaching," J.
103]

P. R. Farnsworth,

104]

R.

W. Brown, "A

Acoust. Soc. Amer., 11 (1940):

(1934):

446-4^0.

"Psychology and Double Stops," Sch. Mus., 2J (1926): 21.
Preliminary Study of the Touch

Method of Learning Piano

Music, "J. Appl. Psychol., 18 (1934): ^16-527.

To

loj]

tie

imagery to action rather than to tones,

F.

Fredrich (Plajing by Seeing,

Medino, Ohio, Lynne, 19^0) suggests the use of note pictures where sketches of the
piano keyboard are placed over the staff and lines are drawn from the piano keys to their
staff notes.

K. L. Bean,

106]

No.

SO,

"An Experimental Approach

6 (1938);

to the Reading of Music," Psychol. Monog.,

"Reading Music Instead of Spelling

"The Use of Visual, Auditory, and Kinesthetic Imagery

It," J.

Musicol.,

in the Transfer of

1

(1939): i-j;

Musical Notation

to the Piano Keyboard," J. Educ. Psjchol., 30 (1939): ^33-^41107]

O. Ortmann, "Span of Vision

in

Note Reading," Mus.

Educ. Nat. Conf. Yearb., 1937,

88-93.
108]

V. Lannert and

Psychol.,

109]

M. Ullman,

"Factors in the Reading of Piano Music," Amer. J.

s8 (1945): 91-99-

H. Lowry,

"On Reading

Music," Diopt. Rev. and

Brit. J.

Phjsiol. Opt.,

I

(1940):

78-88.
1

10]

L. F.

Wheelwright, An Experimental Study of

Symbols, Teach. Coll. Contr.

the Perceptibility

and Spacing of Music

Educ, No. 77^, 1939, N.Y., Teachers

College, Columbia

U., 1939.
Ill]

H.

E.

Weaver, "A Survey of Visual Processes

Musical Selections," Psychol. Monog., S5, No.

i

in

Reading Differently Constructed

(1943): 1-30.

112] K. Van Nuys and H. E. Weaver, "Memory Span and Visual Pauses
Rhythms and Melodies," Psjchol. Monog., SS, No. i (1943): 33-50.

in

Reading

223

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
113]

O.

I.

Jacobsen,

"An

Analytic Study of Eye-movements in Reading Vocal and

Instrumental Music," J. Musical., 3 (1941): 1-32, 69-100, 133-164; 3 (1942): 197-226.
114]

H. Cowell, "The Process of Musical Creation," Awer.

J. Psychol.,

37 (1926): 233-

236.
115]

E.

Benham, "The Creative Activity,"

Brit. J. Psjchol. (Gen.

Sec), 20 (1929): ^9—6^.

For somewhat similar research on children eight to eleven see the

D. Doig. Of special interest

entitled "Creative Music" by

number

is

2

series of articles

which

is

to be

found

J. Educ. Res., 36 (1942): 241-253.

116]

J.

Bahle, Der musikalische Schajfensprozess, Leipzig, Hirzel, 1936.

117] B. Gross and R. H. Seashore, "Psychological Characteristics of Student and Professional Musical

118]

W.

Composers," J. Appl.

G. Whittaker,

W. O.

Psychology of Music and Painting,"
119]
I

20]

Psychol.,

2^ (1941):

Brit. J. Psjchol.,

i

W.

Hutchison, and R.

59-170.
Pickford,

"Symposium on the

33 (1942): 40—57.

C. L. Lastrucci, "The Professional Dance Musician." Thesis, Stanford U., 1941.

H.

S.

Becker, "The Professional Dance Musician and His Audience," Amer.J.

Sociol.,

57 ( 1 9 5 1 ): 1 3 6— 44. A somewhat similar study is being made of contemporary American
composers of "serious" music by D. Nash ("Challenge and Response in the American
1

Composer's Career, "J.
121]

Max

Aesth.,

Graf, From Beethoven

14 (1955): 116— 122).
to

Shostakovich; the Psychology oj the Composing Process,

N.Y.,

Philosophical Library, 1947.
122]

J.

Jancke, "Das Spezifisch-Musikalische und die Frage nach

Musik," Arch.
123]

L. Loar,

Ges. Psjchol.,

dem

Sinngehalt der

J 8 (1930): 103-184.

"An Adventure

in Musical Psychoanalysis," J. Musical., 2,

No.

i

(1940):

15-23.
124]

Anton Ehrenzweig,

The

aj Artistic

Psjcha-analjsis

Vision

and Hearing,

London,

Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1953.
125]

M. L

126]

G. Revesz,

Press,

Stein, "Creativity

and Culture, "J.

Psychol.,

36 (1953): 311-322.

in his Introduction to the Psychology oJ Music,

1954, maintains that there

is

a single

manifests itself in very young children

homogeneous

much

less

Norman, U. of Oklahoma
composing which

talent for

often than does the reproductive-

interpretative talent.

127]

Biographical studies by psychologists are the following:

J.

Bahle, Eingehung und Tat

im musikalischen Schajfen, Leipzig, Hirzel, 1939; H. Jancke, "Beitrage zur Psychologie der

musikalischen Komposition," Arch. Ges. Psjchol., 66 (1928): 437-492; R. Schramek,
(1934): 45—84; P. C. Squires, "The Creative
Psychology of Cesar Franck," Char, and Pers., 7 (1938): 41—49; P. C. Squires, "The

"Franz Liszt," Arch. Ges. Psjchol., g2

Creative Psychology of Chopin," J. Musical., 2, No.

i

(1940): 27-37; P. C. Squires,

"Peter Ilich Tschaikowsky (a psychological sketch)," Psjchaanal. Rev., 28 (1941): 445—

465; P. C. Squires, "The Creative Psychology of Carl Maria von Weber," Char, and Pers.,
6 (1938): 203-217; P. E. Vernon, "The Personality of the Composer," Music and Letters,
II (1930): 38-48.

224

THE NATURE OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
H. C. Lehman, "'Intellectual'

128]

vs.

'Physical Peak'

Performance,"

Sci.

Month., 61

(194J): 127-137.
Bjorksten, "The Limitation of Creative Years,"

129]

J.

130]

W.

Month., 62 (1946): 94.

Dennis, "Predicting Scientific Productivity in Later Maturity from Records of

Earlier Decades," J. Gerontol., 9 (19^4):

H. C. Lehman and D.

131]

Sci.

W.

46^-467.

Ingerham, "Man's Creative Years in Music,"

Sci.

Month.,

48 (1939): 431—443; H. C. Lehman, Age and Achievement, Princeton, N.J., Princeton U.
Press, 19J3.

132]

Most

authorities find

no evidence for

a

unitary creative ability in the arts.

Rev.
J. P. Guilford, "Creative Abilities in the Arts," Psychol.

15

FSP

64 (1957): 110-118.

See

CHAPTER NINE

The Measurement of Musical
Abilities

IVlusiCAL

can be measured in a variety of ways.

abilities

For

persons already proficient in some phase of music, achievement tests
are appropriate. These

may be measures

of appreciation (see Chap. 7),

before the person to be tested has had
In the latter,

are the aptitude tests.

either of verbal knowledge,

or of nonverbal musical

much

music

is

skills.

For use

formal training, there
usually

broken into

various components and tests are constructed in each of these

ponent

areas.

In a sense, aptitude tests are also

ment, although they aim to

its

com-

measures of achieve-

test informal learning

and potential

ability

rather than the effects of formal training.^

Tests

of Verbal Knowledge

Music achievement
geared

to

school

tests

based on verbal knowledge are generally

performance

and

attempt

to

well certain musical abilities have been taught. They

measure
tell

how

us nothing

about what should be taught and so do nothing toward altering

Achievement

the musical status quo.

tests

typically

possess

high

reliability.

The

earliest published tests of verbal

Standardized Music

Tests,

which cover

There are parts devoted
226

a

knowledge are the Beach

wide area of musical

abilities.^

to notation, to the elements of "time

and

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
tune both in isolated form and in melodies," recognition of funda-

mental structural elements, pitch differences, memory, sight singing,
and to the writing of music. The

title is

spmething of

a

misnomer

as

the battery has not been well standardized.

Beach's

battery was followed

by the Musical Achievement

developed by Gildersleeve and Soper.3 This has

designed to

five parts

measure recognition of compositions from notation and

Test

as

played by

the examiner; ability to detect changes in pitch, meter, key signature

and meter signature; knowledge of instrumentation, theory, history,
note values, time signatures, and transpositions from one clef to
another; and ability to use accidentals, to locate
keys, and to write key signatures.
eight,

it

The

la in six different

Designed for grades four through

has a reliability of over -90 at each age-grade.

Torgerson-Fahnestock Music Test

ment measures. 4

A

Part

taps

is

another of the older achieve-

knowledge of note and

rest values,

signatures, pitch and syllable names, expressive marks,

minor key

time

major and

signatures, repeat bars, slurs, do placements, clefs, and

natural and harmonic

minor

Part B tests ear training through

scales.

four subparts which are concerned with the writing of syllable

names, time signatures, and notes,

as

well

as

with the detection of

pitch and time errors.

The most used of the

early

achievement measures was the

Kwalwasser-Ruch Test of Musical Accomplishment.^ This battery attempts
to test the following phases of public-school music

from grades four

through twelve: knowledge of musical terms and symbols, pitch and
letter

names

in bass

and treble

clefs,

time signatures, key signatures,

note values, rest values, and familiar melodies from notation; and
detection of pitch and time errors in a familiar melody.

An

achievement measure very similar in most respects to the

Kwalwasser-Ruch

is

the Strouse Music

areas except for detection of pitch

pitch height and time length.^

Test,

which covers the same

and time errors and adds

The Strouse measure

tests of

takes longer to

227
15-2

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
administer and must be accompanied by a piano or singer. There are
three forms which can be used in grades four through twelve.

Number

of the Hutchinson Music

i

Tests

a

is

measure of tonal

Snatches from ig well-known melodies are presented

imagery. 7

along with the names of these and of 2^ additional songs.

purpose of Hutchinson's measure

Norms

recognition."

The Knuth

"silent

test

reading and

are available for grades seven through twelve.

Achievement Tests in Music are well characterized by their

"For

subtitle,

to

is

Recognition

of

Rhythmic

Certain

and

Melodic

Aspects."^ In presenting the stimuli for the test, the examiner

represent one of the four scores the student has in his hands.

five

first

chord to sound the key and then plays four measures which

strikes a

Knuth

The

The

has three levels, one for grades three and four, another for

and

six,

and the third for grades seven through twelve.

The McCaulej Experiment
four through nine,

Soper, but so

is

in Public School

much

Music, arranged for grades

by Gildersleeve and

like the older test

much longer that

it

takes

more than one

There are measures of knowledge of syllable and

session to give.

letter

names, of note

and rest values, meter and key signatures, chromatics, sight and aural
identification of melodies,

types of compositions, musical instru-

ments, famous names in music, and musical terms.

A somewhat more
in Music. '^^

Its

limited measure

key signatures, measure signatures,
bass-staff syllables,
rivals, it

is

the Providence Inventory Test

ten sections have to do with naming notes, note values,
rest values, syllables, melodies,

and symbols; and placing

has but one form.

It is

do.

Like most of

its

intended for grades four through

nine.

A

rather different kind of achievement test

lary list

developed by

L.

C. Pressey."

The

vocabulary" words appear in capital letters,

mentary words

in italics,

found in textbooks are
228

is

the musical vocabu-

so-called "fundamental
fairly

important supple-

and words of no great importance but often

in ordinary type.

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
Ear

Tests in

Harmony are measures suitable for use in any standard

course in harmony. ^^ They test "active musical experience in the

world of sound." Sound combinations of many

sorts including the

rather unusual are treated in this battery.

Among

the

more

recently published achievement batteries

devised by Kotick and Torgerson.^3 Their Diagnostic

Tests

ment in Music are intended for grades four through twelve.

covered include diatonic

syllable,

chromatic

syllable,

one

is

of Achieve-

The

topics

and number

names; time signatures; major and minor keys; note and rest values;
names;

letter

A

signs

and symbols; key names; and song recognition.

contemporary measure which

Tarnum Music Notation
sented by means of

a

Test.^^

is

enjoying some success

is

the

Forty four-measure melodies are pre-

phonograph record. The student follows along

with a printed score and selects for each melody the one measure in

which the

norms are

pitch, rhythm, or time

is

handled differently.

available for grades seven, eight,

The

sexes separately.

Sets of

and nine, and for the two

test correlates appreciably

with

a

number of

the standard measures of musical aptitude and with scores on certain

instrumental performance scales.

Another contemporary measure
Test,

whose

is

the

Aliferis

Music Achievement

tonal stimuli can be given either by piano performance or

tape recording. ^5 Intended for use at the college freshman level, this
test has

melodic, harmonic, and rhythmic sections. The total test

said to correlate

reliable

with music grades in the

'^^o's

and

•6o's.

It is

is

quite

and has been extremely well standardized with separate

T-scores for each of the four major geographical sections of the

United
In

1

9

States.
5" I

a

much used

music achievement

test

was added to the well known and

Graduate Record Examination, Advanced. ^^ Achievement

is

measured from the senior year of college through graduate school.
Since this battery

is

part of the Institutional Testing Program,

available for general use

it is

not

and cannot be described here.
229

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
Tests

The

of Nonverbal Musical

Skills

earliest of the semistandardized tests of musical

performance

(1923) was the Hillbrand Sight-Singing

Test,

a

fourth-, fifth-, and sixth-grade pupils/7

The

test contains six songs

which the student

studies for a

few minutes and then

The examiner

help or accompaniment.

measure devised for

listens

wrongly pitched, to transpositions, notes

without

sings

with an ear to notes
notes sharped,

flatted,

notes omitted, errors in time, extra notes, repetitions and hesitations.
Hillbrand' s scale was followed
Individual Singing. ^^

Twelve

two

years later by the Mosher Test of

exercises, arranged in order of difficulty,

are presented and sung back by the pupil.

measures

rendered tonally and

Mosher was co-author of

a

The score

rhythmically

is

the tally of the

correct.

In

0-M

Sight-Singing Test.^^ This

one.

The items "progress through most of the major keys and

measure

is

structurally like the earlier

duce the minor mode. Some of the exercises
test

1932

second performance scale entitled the

intro-

in the latter part of the

begin on scale steps other than the tonic." The test has high

reliability

(odd-even) and

two examiners

reasonably objective in the sense that any

is

will agree fairly well

on the

ratings

which should be

given.

Watkins's A Performance
a scale

Test

for the Cornet or Trumpet consists in

of fourteen melodic exercises.^"

obtained, one as the test

is

Two

scores are usually

administered to the pupil

at sight,

and the

other after he has had a week's time to practice the material. The
scores are built up from a tabulation of pitch, time, change of tempo,

expression, and slur errors; and the mishandling of rests, holds,
pauses, and repeats.

cornet or trumpet
latter

may be used

An
is

adaptation of this performance scale for the

Watkins-Farnum Performance

The

for any

Scale."^^

This

band instrument. The fourteen exercises

are so graded that while the

first is

intended for those

who

have had

lessons for approximately three months, the fourteenth will be found

230

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
difficult

by the student

reliability

is

good

who

as is also

the correlation with teacher ratings.

Relatively unstandardized achievement tests keep appearing

time to time.^^

A few

of these

owe

construct

test battery,

music achievement

tests

do not meet the standards of good
fluid

and uncertain

music has made the task of devising
o achievement
difficult

than

there will be

in, say,

more

the scientific areas.

tests in

Perhaps

at

When

this

test

state of school

music more

some

later date

general agreement on what children should

about music by the end of each grade.
first-rate tests

profit

although at present the majority of

Without doubt the

construction.

too local to justify

still

Hence, each large school system can with

own

its

from

their birth to the idea that the

music curricula of the public schools are
national norms.

The

has studied for several years.

know

time arrives, really

can be devised for the area of music.

Unstandardized Aptitude Tests

We

have seen that the music achievement

current curriculum practices and give
basic musical abilities.

little

tests largely reflect

help in isolating the

Attention must be shifted, then, to the

musical aptitude tests in the hope that they

may

yield

more

pertinent

information.

Stumpf was one of the
basic musical abilities. ^3

first

psychologists to interest himself in the

In his

work with

the young genius Pepito

Areola, he stressed the following: possession of absolute pitch;

unusually good pitch and timbre discrimination; excellent musical

memory;

ease in judging pitch intervals; and ability in transposition,

in improvisation, and in producing dissonant chords and series of

"unmelodic" tones. Stumpf saw four basic
tell

whether

a clang

abilities in musicality: to

was composed of one or two tones, to discrimi-

nate pitch differences, to judge degrees of consonance as to pleasantness,

and to sing correctly.

Pear modified Stumpf's ideas very
231

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
slightly

by testing for pitch aptitude,

a clang,

ability to sing, ability to analyze

and consistency in reporting the clang analyses. ^4

number of ingenious devices to assess
musical talents. ^5 His "obe-imeter" measures "how echotheratic
(sound-hunting) the subject is." The task in this test is to listen for
Meyer

has developed a

not easily

an auditory stimulus w^hich

is

masked by other tones and

noises.

follovv^ed since it

is

partially

The "concertometer" shows how

well a musician can play with other musicians in concert. The

"rhythmometer," which resembles the motor rhythm
Seashore independently developed

person can follow

a

(p.

23^), measures

rhythmic pattern and reproduce

it.

test

R. H.

how

well a

The "terpo-

meter" presents major, minor, and mistuned chords which are to be
classified as either active, sad,

battery, the

"hymnometer,"

is

or neutral.

The

in essence a tonal

the "terpometer" and the "hymnometer"

member of the
memory test. Both

last

make

use of a specially

devised quarter-tone reed organ.

During

his early research,

Revesz appeared to regard absolute

Chord and

pitch as the most important element of musicality.
interval recognition; ability to

and keen

memory were

also

compose, improvise, and to transpose;

deemed

to be basic musical abilities.

of these Revesz found to be present
child Nyiregyhazi (p. 185-),
Psychology of a Musical

Prodigy. ^^

do with

fitness for

In

talents, for

more recent

(a) aptitudes

some

talent

The

latter

type

is

subdivided into

and talent for conducting.

have

refer to

Musical

instrumental-

Revesz would
tests

now

of rhythmic

regional pitch, ability to analyze two-tone clangs and

chords, and ability to grasp and sing a melodic line.

232

feels

which

special field. ^7

measure the "lower grades of musicality" through
sensitivity,

theorizing

which he

(h) talents

in The

him, are of two distinct types: creative and reproductive-

interpretative.

virtuoso

his

performance and

capacities far above the average in

All

an early age in the wonder-

whose achievements he described

Revesz has differentiated between
to

at

For the "higher

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
grades of musicality" he

and

give tests suited to the

melodies from memory.

ability to play familiar

For Rupp the

measurement

harmonic apprehension and response, creative

of relative pitch,
fantasy,

would

of basic abilities included: interval recognition,

list

absolute pitch, chord analysis, harmonic feeling, melodic recognition

and reproduction, and
Billroth

^9

ability to

sensitivity to

rhythms and time differences.^^

and von Kries^^ had briefer

lists.

The former's included

remember, recognize, and reproduce short melodies. The

contained sense of rhythm, musical memory, and musical ear.

latter's

For Mjon the

five

most important

abilities

appeared to be: to com-

pose, to possess absolute pitch, to play by ear, to improvise a second
voice, and to sing a second voice. 3^

Haecker and Ziehen obtained the bulk of their data on musical
abilities

by sending out ii,ooo questionnaires. 3^ Exceptional tonal

memory seemed
less

them

to

more from

best to distinguish their

musical respondents.

An

excellent prognostic sign for later

musicality turned out to be precocity in things musical.
vocal

skill, a

skills, e.g.,

motor

ability, to

and

be

little

tests,

one of musical memory, another

must be decided which of two cadences

it

They found

related to the several sensory

pitch discrimination.

Low^ery has devised three

which

their

a phrase test in

which the problem

phrase has been repeated. 33

Ortmann

is

to tell

is

in

more complete,

whether or not

a

has a battery of seven tests

at the Peabody Conservatory of Music. 34 The
memory, rhythm memory, melodic memory,

which has enjoyed use

members

are: pitch

harmonic memory, fusion, pitch discrimination, and time discrimination.

Schoen, too, has offered a battery of

the measurement of relative pitch,
difference in distance

tests. 35

These are for

where one must judge the

between two successive pitch

intervals; tonal

sequence, in which four two-phrase melodies are given with alternative endings; and

rhythm discrimination, where

either be repeated or reappear in

somewhat

a

rhythm may

altered form.

A

tonal

233

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
movement

test of considerable

five unfinished

the best

final

promise

is

Franklin's

TMT. Twenty-

melodies are presented to the testees with requests for
Retest and split-half reliabilities in the

tone for each.

•8o's are reported for an adult music student population.

of the

TMT

it is

poorer students

Among
aptitude

those

is

at a level considerably better

a

than chance. 3^

who have used achievement tests as measures of
who has made an extensive study of the ability

Madison,

to discriminate intervals. 37
all

By the use

apparently possible to select the better from the

musical perception,

it

most important musical

Since interval discrimination

would seem
ability.

that

Scores on Madison's measure have

aptitude by

Lamp and Keys

way of achievement. They
brass

in theory at the

and from "39 to 'ji with indices of musical

at the secondary-school level.

weeks on

basic to

Madison has specialized on

been found to correlate from -46 to 'ji with grades
Juilliard School

is

3^

ability

have also studied

trained pupils for several

then on woodwinds, and later on

instruments,

After these weeks of training, ratings

strings (the

order varied).

were made

for achievement

on each family of instruments. The

authors hoped through such ratings to learn which of the three sorts
of instruments was best suited to the aptitudes of each of their pupils.

They appeared

to enjoy mild success in their prognostications.

These

teachers also attempted, without success, to forecast later achieve-

ment through

analyses of tooth evenness, lip thickness, and length

and slenderness of fingers

No

(p.

188).

well standardized tests have so far been developed to measure

control of pitch intonation, loudness, time, or rhythm.

But a record

of pitch control can be obtained through the use of instruments such
as the

tonoscope39 and other standard stroboscopic devices 4°

the market

which show

instrumental attempts.

dynamic control are

now on

visually the accuracy of a person's vocal or

Intensity meters and instruments to study

available. 4^

Ability to control time and

rhythm

can be studied through the Meyer "rhythmometer," mentioned

234

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
above, or by means of R. H. Seashore's "rhythm meter," which

phonograph with contacts imbedded

A number

points. 4^
subject,

is

a

in the turn-table at various

of different rhythms can be provided for the

who must make his

on

taps

a telegraph

key coincide with the

he hears.

clicks

C. E. Seashore and his students have felt that the ability to imagine
tonal material

is

an important aspect of musical aptitude. 43 Their

of testing imagery
will

Naturally, their
adults.

is

images

elicit

The

to present
in

list

subjects,

the

of questions which,

lists

eight

it is

most important sensory

for children differs

from

way

hoped,
areas.

that appropriate to

whatever their age, must introspectively evalu-

ate the strengths of their images

on

a seven-point scale.

The Original Seashore Battery
The

first really

standardized aptitude tests

C. E. Seashore. 44

menon

were those devised by

Seashore argued that since music was a pheno-

of tones, times, and rhythms, discrimination tests in these

areas should

make

possible to pick out the potentially musical,

it

with those having the best acuities being expected to give the
Seashore believed that his tests tapped

greatest musical promise.

basic physiological capacities

influenced by training.
that there

were other

He

which were inborn and could not be

admitted that his

capacities he

test battery

was limited,

was not measuring.

number of
Many psychologists and musicians have condemned its
and unmusical orientation. 4^ They have emphasized that the

Seashore's test philosophy has been criticized on a
counts. 45

atomistic

Seashore tests get at psychophysiological, and not necessarily at
musical, differences.

What

is

the good, they say, of a performer's

being able to discriminate pitches which differ by
listeners can only discriminate pitches

cycle

if his

more

cycles apart?

If

the performer

is

a pianist,

as little as

which are

one

five

or

why need he bother
23^

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
with differences smaller than a half-tone? Other psychologists have

been made unhappy by the Seashore claim that

improved by practice. 47

native capacity and cannot be

proved possible to entice
hereditarian camp.

test scores reflect

music

all

testers aw^ay

It

pure

has not

from Seashore's

But there has been amassed an impressive array

of data which demonstrate beyond the possibility of doubt that the

Seashore scores can be enormously improved

procedures are employed

The

if

proper training

i86).

(p.

original Seashore Measures of Musical Talent offered scores for

sense of pitch, sense of intensity, sense of time, sense of consonance,

and auditory

memory

span (tonal memory). 4^ Six years later a sense

Because of the many criticisms of

of rhythm measure was added.

the sense of consonance test was later dropped (p. 49).

each

member

of the battery were

eighth-graders, and for fifth-graders,

made

Norms

available for adults,

who were

it,

for
for

thought to be the

youngest age group which could properly attend to the tonal stimuli
of the

tests.

There have been

a

number

of attempts to improve the battery.

Several have found that the norms, at least for the intensity test,

vary

somewhat with the type of phonograph employed. Others have

advocated simplifying the directions, which are quite
fifth-graders to

comprehend. 49 Salisbury and Smith have modified

the pitch test so that
difficult, instead

all

of the items are scaled from less to

of from less to

original measure. 5*^

the aim of adapting

more and then more

Hattwick has
it

dropped the most

changes did improve the
tests are

test

tests

grades.

were too

items of these

5^

with

Gaw,

difficult for

tests. 5^

These

somewhat, although the Gaw-modified

easy. 53

have been developed, since

make
236

now a little too

tests

memory
difficult

first five

more

to less as in the

also modified the pitch test

to children of the

deciding that the time and tonal
fifth-graders,

difficult for

California adult
it

norms for the rhythm

West Coasters
do lowans.54 O'Connor

has been found that

better scores on this measure than

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
would modify the
difficulty,

memory

tonal

by (i) better scaling of item

test

(2) greater temporal spacing between items, and (3) the

use of the whole-tone as the minimal interval. 55

Tilson-Gretsch Test for Musical Aptitude
Created to replace or

at least to serve as a

second form of the

original Seashore battery, the Tilson-Gretsch tests played into
in appearing shortly after the revised

bad luck

(1939) Seashore battery. 5^ The

Tilson-Gretsch presents tests in the areas of pitch, intensity and time
sensitivities,

and tonal memory.

records whereas

recorded on two phonograph

It is

older rival was spread over

its

six.

considerably more communicable than Seashore's but
easy.
fifth-

Its reliabilities

are approximately the

same

directions are

Its
its

items are too

as Seashore's at the

and eighth-grade levels but are considerably poorer in the adult

range. This battery

1^39

now

has

little

excuse for continued existence. 57

Edition of the Seashore Measures

oj Musical Talents
Only minor changes
in

this

newer

"talents."

in the Seashore test philosophy are reflected

battery. 5^

Musical

is

measure consonance. The term "intensity"
appropriate "loudness."
sufficient
it.

importance to

become musical

no longer an attempt to
is

replaced by the

Sensitivity to timbre

is

recognized

justify the construction of a test to

more
as

of

measure

But the bulk of the objections raised in connection with the

earlier edition

still

pertain.

Two

forms of the battery,

constructed, one an easier and the other a

Form B was
are

has

"talent"

As was mentioned above, there

now

later

withdrawn from

available for

Form A



sale.

more

Three

A

and B, were

difficult series,

sets

but

of centile norms

for the fourth and fifth grades, for

the sixth, seventh, and eighth, and for grades nine through sixteen.

237

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
The

basic tones of the pitch test are set at ^oo cycles. 59

the go items in the test consist of
pitch.
is

The

subject's task

17 cycles,
finally to

moves
2

The

first.

to differences of

The

cycles.

Brown) ranges from

differ only in

whether the second of each pair

to state

is

higher or lower than the

two tones which

Each of

i

test

opens with differences of

cycles, then to 8, g, 4, 3,

2

and

by Spearman-

reliability (odd-even, raised

'79 in the lower grades to '88 for adults.

The go items of the loudness

test are also

Here

arranged in pairs.

the subject must decide whether the second tone of each pair

weaker or stronger than the
is

from four decibels

in the

first.

The range of loudness

to a half-decibel.

lower grades to '88

The

is

at the adult level.

more

The time



3

is

about equally reliable

Each item of the rhythm

first.

second, the test becomes progres-

difficult: "2, -i^, '12^, -lo, 'Oj g,

test

whether the second

held a longer or shorter time period than the

Starting with differences of
sively

differences

run from '79

reliabilities

Similarly, in the time test the subject judges

tone of each pair

is

and

finally 'o^^

second.

at all ages ('76).

test consists in

two patterns which may

have either identical or different rhythms. With only 30 true-false
items, the reliability of this test

the poorest of

is

junior-high-school levels (-62 and '69) and

is

all at

the adult and

second lowest

at the

grade-school level ('73).

The measure of timbre

more

also uses the same-different

items than the rhythm

To change

test.

scheme but has

the timbre, the intensity

of the tone's fourth partial was increased and that of the third partial
w^as

decreased by an amount necessary to keep the total intensity

constant. In the

The changes
^•5-,

and

first fifth

in the

finally

more

of the go items this change
difficult

of 4-0 decibels.

The measure of tonal memory
choice plan.

Hence,

items are

The

first

of

is

of i o decibels.

8' g,

reliability ranges

then of 7*0,

from -69 to -77.

has 30 items and follows a multiple-

its reliability is

high (from '84 to -SB).

Short

series of tones are given and then repeated with one of the tones of

238

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
each series changed in pitch.

One-third of the series are three-tone

sequences, another third are four-tone, and the remaining third are

While the Seashore

five-tone.

independent of one another,

tests

the

are,

tonal

in the main,

memory

test

relatively

correlates

appreciably with the others.

VALIDITY OF SEASHORE BATTERIES. The majority of the
many validity studies on these widely used tests have been concerned
with the
against

set of records Seashore

which the Seashore

tests

brought out in 19 19. The criteria

have been measured are several, with

teacher ratings and grades in music appearing in perhaps most of the
studies.

Where

—and occahave been reported— they tend

correlations of any size have been found

sionally values in the •6o's

and 'yo's

to be the highest for the tonal

memory and

pitch tests ^° and for the

battery considered as a single measuring instrument. ^^

In

one of the

few studies of the 1939 tests. Manor found that fourth-grade work
instrumental music could be forecast by the pitch test with
coefficient of correlation of -49

and by the measure of tonal

in

a

memory

with a correlation value of '32.^^
Intelligence tests have

been found to be of more worth than the

Seashore tests in forecasting music grades in academic classes, e.g.,

more
more the classes are tonally conducted, e.g., classes in
harmony. ^3 Members of musical organizations make, in the main,
higher than average Seashore scores. ^4 High scorers show a greater

history and appreciation of music, while the Seashore are of

value the

preference for "classical" music than do the lower scorers says one
study, ^5 while, a.ccording to another, their preferences l5an

toward the "romantic
Persons

who

classical" (as

regard themselves as

opposed to

more

"light classical"). ^^

more musical than

average tend to

achieve significantly higher music test scores, particularly in tonal

memory, than those who think of themselves

as

below the musical

average. ^7

239

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
From
have

the discussion so

at

least

some

far, it is

vaUdity.

apparent that the Seashore

tests

Further support comes from data

gathered at the Eastman Conservatory of Music, ^^ w^here the music
tests (19 19 edition)

were added

to a

measure of academic inteUi-

gence, a case history, and a test of tonal imagery, and the combined
scores w^ere used for selection purposes.
studied.

was found

It

1

7

per cent of the bottom

In a slightly later study at Eastman,

as successful.

that course grades in musical theory correlated
scores. ^9

were

that 60 per cent of the persons in the top fifth

succeeded in graduating, but only

were

Five levels of scores



5^9

it

fifth

was shown

with Seashore

Less success was achieved with these music tests at the

College of Music of Cincinnati, which concluded that although

music

tests are of

some

use, they should always

junction with academic intelligence

tests. 7°

be employed

in con-

McLeish, a British

psychologist, suggests that appreciation tests should also be used

along with the Seashore battery. 7^

Kwalwasser-Dykema Music
The Kwalwasser-Dykema
only serious
It

rival,

Tests

battery, for a long time the Seashore's

attempts to do

all its

competitor does and more.?^

has measures for the Seashore-tested areas of pitch, intensity, time,

quality (timbre), rhythm, and tonal

appreciation

Chap.

7).

— melodic

taste

memory. There

are

two

tests

and tonal movement (described in

In addition, there are tests of pitch

and rhythm imagery,

which are measures of achievement rather than of aptitude. The
are

much briefer than

of

the Seashore, the test items seem

tests

more musical,

and the directions are easier to follow. But, except for the measures
of tonal

memory and

tonal

movement, the

reliabilities

are very

low. 73 This poor reliability accounts in part for the fact that only the
tonal

memory

The
240

test correlates at all

well with

its

Seashore counterpart.

structural arrangement of the individual tests

is

as follows.

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
The measure

of tonal

memory

consists of

2

5^

pairs of patterns

which

range in length from four to nine tones. The patterns are repeated
either in original or in altered form, and the subject responds with

the words "same" or "different."
presents 30 items, each

The

test of quality discrimination

composed of two tones played on some

instrument and then repeated on that or a different

particular

Like the previous

instrument.

the responses "same"

test, it calls for

or "different." The stimuli of the time-discrimination test were

recorded from a player-piano

Twenty-five tones lasting from

roll.

•03 to -30 second are repeated unchanged or with a different dura-

rhythmic patterns repeated in the same or in an

tion. Twenty-five

form make up the rhythm-discrimination

altered

test.

The K-D

intensity-discrimination test offers 30 tones and chords and then

repeats

them

at different intensities.

strengths of the second

members

nation test there are 40 items.
3

seconds, rises or

falls

in pitch

imagery and rhythm-imagery
hear with what
items

is

subjects judge the relative

of the pairs.

In the pitch-discrimi-

Each tone, held for approximately
and then returns to

The pitch changes range from

position.

25-

The

tests,

'6 to

5-0

its

original pitch

d.v.

In the pitch-

the subjects compare what they

on the printed blanks supplied them. Each of the

to be described as "same" or "different. "74

is

Several persons have been disturbed by the fact that only one set of

norms

is

that

persons, eight or eighty, trained or untrained, score similarly.

all

offered for this battery. 75 Apparently the test designers feel

But that several

who

is

The

sets of

at all familiar
reliabilities of

norms are needed should not

with the area of aptitude

surprise anyone

testing.

approximately half of the members of the

battery have been materially improved by Holmes,

who

K-D

has changed

the plan of the tests from true-false to multiple-choice. 7^ This he did
solely

by altering the directions for administering the several

measures.

memory

Thus,

test

the subject taking the

Holmes-modified tonal-

must not only decide whether there has been

a

change
241

16

FSP

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
in pattern but, in addition,

of each item

is,

if

possible

responses

"equal,"

"different,"

for

the

"different,"

quality-discrimination

as last

The

are

now

test

of intensity discrimination

"different

they are

and weaker," and "different and

stronger;" for the tonal-movement measure,

"same

first.

and heavier," and "different and

"different

For the measure

lighter."

"equal,"

must check whether the second pattern

changed, higher or lower than the

"down," and

"up,"

note actually heard;" for the measure of time discrimi-

nation, "equal," "different," "different and longer," and "different

and

shorter;"

for

the

test

of rhythm

discrimination,

"equal,"

been changed,"

"different," "different because the time values have

and "different because the accent has been changed;" for the pitchdiscrimination measure, "equal," "different," "different and higher,"

and "different and lower;" and for the melodic-taste

test,

"B better," and "equally good." With these changes, the
the battery

now

There are

employed
training,

a

"A

better,"

reliability of

reaches '91 (high-school level).

number

of studies in which the

K-D

battery has been

to forecast teacher ratings and grades in sight-singing, ear-

and in

"all fine arts"

courses.

In

some

instances there has

been little or no success reported, but in one study the astonishingly
high correlation value of '83 has been claimed. 77 Perhaps the modal
forecast value for the battery as a

hood of

whole would

lie in

the neighbor-

'40, with that for the individual tests being considerably

lower.

Kwalwasser Music Talent Tests
In

195^3

there appeared a

new Kwalwasser

requires only ten minutes to take. 7^

more
norms

difficult

Accompanying Form A, the

of the test's two phonograph records,

for junior-high-school students

high and college groups.

242

aptitude test which

Form B

is

and another

is

one

set of

set for senior-

for grades 4, ^, and 6.

Form A

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
"consists of ^o three-tone patterns
in

which are repeated with

one of the following respects:

(J) loudness."

The pitch

tempo changes from 40

(a) pitch,

differences range

(b) time,

from

5^

rhythm,

to 70 cents, the

to g per cent of the standard

marking of 90 to the quarter-note, and loudness

(c)

variation

varies

metronomic
from 10 to

2

The rhythmic changes become more

decibels from the standard.

difficult as the test progresses.

The corresponding

values for the

40-item Form B are: pitch, 15^—70 cents; time, 15^—40 per cent; and
loudness, 3—10 decibels.

The rhythms proceed from more

changes in pattern.

difficult

to less

Kwalwasser, a staunch hereditarian and

student of C. E. Seashore, closes his eyes to the research literature

and maintains that training can have

made on

his test.

little

or no effect on the scores

Kwalwasser has violated approved

test practice

by

publishing a commercial test without reliability data of any kind.

The Drake Tests
Drake offered the musical world four partially standardized

In 1932

aptitude

tests. 79

memory),

These covered the areas of musical

interval discrimination, retentivity

memory (melody

(memory

for isolated

tones), and intuition (key center, phrase and time balance).

he replaced these with two well standardized

tests,

one

In 195^4

in the area

musical

memory and the other to forecast rhythmic ability. ^° The
memory test has two equivalent forms, A and B, and the

rhythm

test has

of musical

an easier

Form A and

musically nai've are tested on

or

more

rhythm

all

a

more

difhcult

B.

The

four forms, but subjects with five

years of musical training need take only

test

Form

Form B

of the

and either form of the measure of musical memory. The

two new Drake

tests intercorrelate

With reliabilities in
scores. Norms for them

only slightly.

the high -So's or low •90's, they yield stable

are available for music and non-music students, and for ages 11, 12,

and 13; 14,

I

^,

and 16; 17, 18, and 19; 20, 21, and 22; and 23 and
243

16-2

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
over.

Sex differences in scores and those associated with

cultural

background are

test

and

be negligible.

said to

The musical memory

racial

made up

is

composed

of especially

melodies, which are either repeated or changed with respect to time,

The rhythm

key, or note.

test presents the

sound of

a

metronome

beating at a certain rate and a voice which counts "one," "two,"
"three," and "four."
at a different rate

The voice then

His score

original rate until told to stop.

the

metronome

beat

Drake reports

number and

a coefficient

scores on his measure of

discrimination.

either stops counting or proceeds

while the subject continues to count silently

his

is

own

the difference between

silent count.

of correlation of only

rhythm and on the Seashore

The two memory

tests,

similar variables and intercorrelate

at the



1

test

7

between

of rhythm

however, are tapping more

at -^^^

(from unpublished data

gathered at Stanford University). Drake's recent studies suggest that
his

new

tests

the fact that

have considerable

when

validity.

He draws

his

support from

correlations are run aaainst
teacher estimates of
O

musical talent, the values are generally above -^8 and have run

high

as

as '91.

Whistler-Thorpe Musical Aptitude Test
The developers of the

Whistler-Thorpe Musical Aptitude Test pride

themselves on presenting musical, not just tonal, stimuli (piano
music). ^^ The test has two "same-different" sections dealing with

rhythm recognition, one

more

difficult.

fairly

easy,

and the other considerably

The "same-different" pattern

melody-recognition portion.

is

also followed in the

In the pitch-discrimination section,

chords are given and then are repeated either with precisely the

same structure or
test

higher or lower pitch level. The section of the

devoted to pitch recognition

pitch

244

at a

is

is

the most novel.

strongly emphasized. Then there follows

Here
a

a particular

melody of four

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
measures of 13 quarter-notes. The subject's task

is

to count the

number of times the previously emphasized pitch has appeared.
The Whistler-Thorpe has a reported reUabiUty of '93. It claims to
correlate at '78 with teacher estimates of vocal talent.

Lundin Tests
measure musical behaviors not considered by the

In an effort to

Lundin devised

earlier testers,

of these measures

is

five rather different tests. ^^

in the area of interval discrimination.

ascending and descending intervals
either repeated without

rendition.

is

in

make up

first

Fifty

the stimuli, which are

change or are modified in the second

The responses of

Lundin battery are

The

this

and the other members of the

terms of "same" or "different." The

reliability

reported to be in the -yo's.

The melodic-transposition

test offers 30

the second rendition the key alone

by "same." But
response

is

if

simple melodies.

If

on

changed, the subject responds

is

both the key and the melody are altered, the proper

"different."

The

reliability

is

said to

be -6^ for musicians

and -72 for unselected college students.

Mode

discrimination

of chords.
is

"same."

"different."

If

the pair

But

if

When

the

is

tested through the presentation of 30 pairs

members are in the same mode, the response
mode is changed, the subject responds with

employed on unselected students,

the poorest reliability of any

For musicians the

member

reliability value

is

this test has

of the battery (one of -lo).

in the

middle

Each of the 30 items of the melodic-sequences

•6o's.

test contains three

melodic groups which follow the same melodic order.

group may or may not follow

this pattern.

The

A

fourth

reliability of this

measure

falls

member

of the battery, the measure of rhythmic sequences, except,

in the •70's.

This test sets the pattern for the

last

of course, that in the latter test rhythms and not melodies are

24^

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
For musicians the rhythm

involved.

unselected college students the value

is

reliability coefficients for the battery as a

In

one study by Lundin, when

for forecasting pooled ratings

correlation

values

The corresponding

-72.

whole are '89 and

total battery scores

made by

emerged:

rehabiUty of -60; for

test has a

-Sg.

were employed

six professors, the following

melodic

dictation,

harmonic

-70;

dictation, '70; written harmonization, -43; general ability in theory,

•63; performance, -^i; and

sum of

Seashore pitch, rhythm, and tonal

memory measure

(original

Wing Standardized
A British flavor can

Lundin found the

ratings, '69.

memory

tests

and Drake's musical

form) to forecast these

criteria less well.

of Musical Intelligence
be seen in the Wing tests, although the
measures are not so different from the much condemned American
tests as one might guess from a perusal of the Wing monograph. ^3
Tests

Recorded on ten records are seven
range

low

as

Five-step

as -Gg,

norms

seventeen.

The

Although their

tests.

the battery reliability

is

are available for each year
selection of the tests

studying

and

who had

at

or so.

from age eight through

was based on factor

That the battery has considerable validity was shown
333 adolescent boys

reliabilities

a satisfactory '93

analyses.

in a study of

one time studied or were

some musical instrument. Wing gave

classified their scores into three classes.

still

his tests to these lads

He found

that

40 per

cent of the below-average group had already given up studying. The

comparable values for the average and above-average groups were
27 and

2

percent, respectively.

Wing's chord-analysis

number

test

requires

the

subject

of notes in a series of single chords.

to

In the

count the

measure of

pitch change, chords are played and repeated either with exactly
similar structure or with

The memory
246

some one note pitched higher or lower.

measure presents tunes which are either repeated or

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
altered.

If

they are modified, the subject must
In the test of

altered notes.

which are the

tell

rhythmic accent, melodies are either

repeated in identical form or with the accents rearranged. The
subject notes the changes,

The

the tune.

except that

any, and states

which versions better

fit

intensity and phrasing tests are similarly arranged

it is

The harmony

if

now

the intensity or the phrasing which

test presents tunes

altered.

is

which are either repeated exactly

or altered. Each item must be assessed for identity or else a decision

must be made

as to the better

of the

two

versions.

Strong Vocational Interest Test

A

rather different sort of test and one

aptitude measure

known

is

is

only in part an

the Strong musician scale. ^4

Strong's well-

set of questions

men whose

which

on

interests has for

some time had

composed of 2^0 musicians,

criterion group was

symphony performers. Their average age was 32*6 and
schooling i2'4.

The

a scale for

scale's reliability

is

-87.

The

largely

their years of

test score indicates

the degree of resemblance between the answer profile of the person
tested and that of the criterion group.

Judged by the high

that the Strong scales achieve in other areas

where

validity

tively easy to ascertain, e.g., life insurance, Strong's

validity
rela-

is

musician scale

merits respectful attention.

During 1952, the Music Journal gathered data for four other Strong
musician

scales.

It

men

tested ^00

schooling, 17*0) to form one of

another 4^0

performer"

men

its

(average age, 41*4;

two "music teacher"

years of

scales

and

(average age, 36*4; education, 13*6) for a "music

scale.

In an

the Journal tested 4.^0

attempt to build parallel scales for women,

women

years of schooling,

16' g)

years of schooling,

i^''^^).

music teachers (average age, 43*8;

and 290 performers (average age, 34*2;

Although the project

is

not

as yet

com-

pleted, the scales are available to anyone interested.

247

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
The Future of Music Aptitude Tests
It

would be

music

foolish to attempt too precise a forecast of the course

tests will take in the

the testers will

make

next few years.

But

it

seems clear that

increasing use of musical materials and that

their tests will not be as atomistic as are the Seashore measures.

More
cast

than a half-century ago, the intelligence testers tried to fore-

achievement by the use of simple items

They then moved
i.Q.

tests,

and

omnibus or buckshot approach with

to an

later

still

at the sensory level.

worked out

sort

a

their

of compromise of

these extremes with their measures of primary mental abilities.

The music

The progressive

sensory materials.

demned
did

this

little

began their labors with the aid of simple

testers, too,

to

educators, in particular, con-

approach and argued for the omnibus stand; yet they

make

model. The philosophy of the more

tests in this

recent testers, however, would seem to allow as test items at
least a

modicum

of sensory material as well as bits of real musical

behavior.

Musical

memory

has

come

to be the one area

agree merits the most careful attention.

may not be important
earlier, the violinist

Other

all

testers

ability areas

may or

As was mentioned

to the potential performer.

needs

which

far better pitch discrimination

than does

the pianist, yet neither instrumentalist should necessarily score near
the top of the Seashore range.

Music

testers

must get away from the

philosophy of "better score, better musical potential." They should
follov^ the course blazed

more

by the college aptitude

minimum

intensively the

the several kinds of musical

testers

and study

levels necessary for later success in

skills.

More and more

they can be

expected to add to their batteries measures of academic aptitude,
interest,

personality,

and perhaps even

base to their forecasting efforts.

and
248

less

attention

to

It is

taste,

to give a broader

likely that they will pay less

the nature- nurture problem.

Instead,

the

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
emphasis will be turned toward the practical issue of deciding

which persons

will profit most, given a

minimum

of practice oppor-

tunities.

Notes
i]

A number

of paper-and-pencil tests in music give no publication dates.

interest of uniformity,
2]

no dates will be given here even when

Beach and H. E. Schrammel, Beach Standardized Music

F. A.

Hence,

in the

available.

Emporia, Kans.,

Tests,

Bureau of Ed. Meas., Kansas State Normal School.
3]

W.

G. Gildersleeve and

Soper, Music Achievement

Tests,

N.Y., Bureau of Publications,

Teachers College, Columbia U.
4]

T. L. Torgerson and E. Fahnestock, Torgerson-Fahnestock Music

Test,

Bloomington,

111.,

Public School Publ.
^]

J.

Kwalwasser and G. Ruch, Kwalwasser-Ruch

of Musical Accomplishment, Iowa City,

Tests

Iowa, Bureau of Ed. Res. and Serv., State U., Iowa.
6]

C. E. Strouse and H. E. Schrammel, Strouse Music

Test,

Emporia, Kans., Bureau Ed.

Meas., Kansas State Teach. Coll.
7]

H. E. Hutchinson, Hutchinson Music

8]

W.

9]

C.

Tests,

Bloomington,

111.,

Public School Publ.

E. Knuth, Knuth Achievement Tests in Music, Philadelphia, Ed. Test Bureau.

McCauley, McCauley Experiment

J.

in Public School Music,

Knoxville, Tenn.,

J. E.

Avent.
10]

R. D. Allen,

W.

H. Butterfield, and M. TuUy, Providence

Inventory Test in Music,

Yonkers, N.Y., World Book.
11]

L. C. Pressey, The Specialized Vocabularies of the Public School Subjects,

111.,

Public School Publ.

12]

C. P.

13]

M.

Wood,

L.

Ear

Tests in

Bloomington,

Harmony, N.Y., American Book.

Kotick and T. L. Torgerson, Diagnostic

Tests

of Achievement in Music, Los

Angeles, Calif. Test Bureau.

Famum, Farnum

Music Notation

14]

S. E.

i^]

Aliferis, Alijeris Music Achievement Test,
J.

Test,

N.Y., Psychological Corp.
Minneapolis, U. of Minnesota Press.

16]

Graduate Record Examinations, Advanced Music

17]

E. K. Hillbrand, Hillbrand Sight-Singing Test, Yonkers, N.Y.,

18]

R. M. Mosher, Mosher

Test

Test,

Princeton, N.J., Ed. Test. Service.

World Book.

of Individual Singing, Teach. Coll. Contrib.

Educ, No.

194,

N.Y., Bureau of Publications, Teachers Coll., Coliambia U.
19]

A.

W.

Otterstein and R.

M. Mosher, 0-M

Sight-Singing

Test,

Stanford,

Calif.,

Stanford U. Press.

249

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
2o]

J.

G. Watkins, Objective Measurement of Instrumental Performance, Teach. Coll. Contrib.
Publications, Teachers Coll., Columbia U.

Educ, No. 860, N.Y., Bureau of
21]

G. Watkins and

J.

S.

E.

Farnum, The Watkins-Farnum Performance

Scale,

Winona,

Minn., Leonard Music.
See,

22]

for

Achievement
Tests,

example,

Plymouth

Educational

Tests,

Chicago,

Plymouth Press; Music

Baltimore, Dept. Ed.; Krone Recognition, Tonal and Rhjthmic Dictation

Test,

Indianapolis,

M.

T. Krone, Butler U.; Mosher Group

Educ. No. 194, N.Y., Columbia U.

Tests,

Teach. Coll. Contrib.

See also A. Roe, "A Study of the Accuracy of

Perception of Visual Musical Stimuli," Arch. Psychol., 1^8 (1933); F. S. Salisbury and
H. D. Smith, "Prognosis of Sight Singing Ability of Normal School Students," J. Appl.
(1929): 42^-439; T. G. Stelzer, "Construction, Interpretation, and Use of

Psychol., 13

Sight Reading Scale in

Organ Music, with an Analysis of Organ Playing

Abilities," J. Exp. Ed.,

7 (1938): 35'-43;

S.

into

Fundamental

T. Burns, "The Value of Prognostic Tests for

Instrumental Pupils," Sch. Mus., 31 (193 i): 6—9.
23]

C. Stumpf, "Akustische Versuche mit Pepito- Areola," Zsch. f. Ang. Psychol., 21

(1909): i-ii.
24]

T. H. Pear, "The Classification of Observers as 'Musical' and 'Unmusical',"

J. Psychol.,

2j]

M.

F.

4 (191

1

):

Brit.

89—94.

Meyer, "Special Ability Tests

as

Used

in Missouri; Including a

Demonstration

of a Typical Test," Psychol. Bull., 21 (1924): 114-116.
26]

G. Revesz, The Psychology of a Musical Prodigy, N.Y., Harcourt, Brace, 192^.

27]

G. Revesz, Introduction

28]

H. Rupp, "Uber die Priifung musikalischer Fahigkeiten," Zsch.

to the

Psychology of Music,

Norman, U. of Oklahoma

f

Press,

Ang. Psychol., 9

(1919): 1-76.
29]

T. Billroth, Wer

30]

J.

31]

J.

Psychol.,

32]

ist

von Kries, Wer

musikalisch?

ist

musikalisch?

Gebriider Paetel, 1898.
Berlin, Springer,

1926.

A. Mjon, "Zur psychologischen Bestimmung der Musikalitat," Zsch. f.
27 (1926): 217-273.

Ang.

V. Haecker and T. Ziehen, Zur Vererbung and Entwicklung der musikalischen Begabung,

Leipzig, Barth, 1923.
33]
1 1

H. Lowery, "Cadence and Phrase Tests

in

Music,"

Brit. J.

Psychol., IJ

(1926-27):

i-i 18.

34]

O. Ortmann,

Tests

of Musical Talent, Baltimore, Peabody Conservatory of Music,

unpublished.
3j]

M. Schoen,

"Tests of Musical Feeling and Musical Understanding," J. Comp. Psychol.,

5 (192^): 31-52.
36]

E. Franklin,

Gumperts

2^0

Tonality as a Basis for the Study of Musical Talent,

Forlag, 19^6.

Goteborg, Sweden,

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
37]

T. H. Madison, "Interval Discrimination as a Measure of Musical Aptitude," Arch.

No. 268 (1942).

Psychol,
38]

C.

J.

Lamp and N.

Keys, "Can Aptitude for Specific Musical Instruments Be Pre-

dicted?" J. Educ. Psychol., 26 (1935^): ^87-596.
39]

C. E. Seashore, "The Tonoscope and

Musician, 11

(1906):

(1928): 166—170; P.

J. Gen. Psychol., 2 (1929):

40]

41] O.

I.

Use

in the Training of the Voice,"

The

S5^~SS^-

Kwalwasser, Exploring

J.

Its

331-332; E. W. Scripture, "Das Strobilion," Zsch.J. Psychol., 59
R. Famsworth, "Two Independent Developments of the Strobilion,"

the Musical

Mind, N.Y., Coleman-Ross, 19^5^, Chap. 10.

Jacobsen, "Dynamic and Temporal Control in Music," j. Gen. Psychol., 1£

(1936): 171-190.
42]

R. H. Seashore, "Studies in Motor Rhythm,"

la. Stud. Psychol.,

9 (1926): 142-199;

"A Study in the Seashore Motor Rhythm Test," Psychol. Monog., 40 (1930):
J.
74-84. For a note on the Sievers rhythm test see H. M. Williams, "A Study in the
Prediction of Motor Rhythmic Performance of School Children," J. Genet. Psychol., 43
T. Nielsen,

(1933): 377-388.
43]

C. E. Seashore,

Chap.

1

The Psychology of Musical

Talent,

Boston, Silver,

Burdett,

19 19,

See also G. H. Smith, "Auditory Imagery in Music Reading," Thesis, Stanford

1.

U., 1947.
44]

C. E. Seashore, Manual oj Instructions and Interpretations Jor Measures of Musical Talent,

N.Y., Columbia Graphophone, 19 19.
45^]

H. Lowery,

1-14;

"On

the Integrative Theory of Musical Talent," J. Musical., 2 (1940):
Erit. J. Educ. Psychol., IJ

Mainwaring, "The Assessment of Musical Ability,"

J.

(1947): 83-96.
46]

The

leader of this group was J. L. Mursell.

See his The Psychology of Music, N.Y.,

Norton, 1937.
47]

R. Wyatt, "A Note on the Use of 'Omnibus' Training to Validate Seashore's

Capacity Hypothesis," Amer.J. Psychol., S2 (1939): 638-640.
48]

C. E. Seashore, Manual of Instructions and Interpretations Jor Measures of Musical Talent,

N.Y., Columbia Graphophone, 19 19. For a survey through 1930 of the research articles on
this first set of

Seashore measures see P. R. Farnsworth,

"An Historical,

mental Study of the Seashore-Kwalwasser Test Battery," Genet.

Critical,

and Experi-

Psychol. Monog.,

9 (193

i):

291-393.
49]

E.

M. McGinnis,

"Seashore's Measures of Musical Ability Applied to Children of the

Pre-school Age," Amer.J. Psychol., 40 (1928): 620-623.
£o]

F. S. Salisbury

and H. B. Smith, "Prognosis of Sight Singing Ability, "J. Appl.

Psychol.,

13 (1929): 42^-439£i]

M.

S.

Hattwick, "Manual of Instructions and Interpretations for

tion Test for
£2\

E. A.

Young Children,"

Gaw, "Five

U. of

la. Stud. Child.

Welf., 11

a Pitch

Discrimina-

(1935): 69-74.

Studies of the Music Tests," Psychol. Monog., 39 (1928): 14^-1^6.

2^1

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
Psychology of Tone and Music," Genet. Psychol.

P. R. Farnsworth, "Studies in the

^3]

Monog., IS (1934): 4^—49; "Further Notes on the Seashore Music Tests, "J. Gen. Psychol.,
18 (1938): 429-431.

M.

^4]

E.

Brown, "A Note Concerning the Seashore Measures of Musical Talent,"

Sch.

and Soc, 30 (1929): 274-27^.
££]

J.

O'Connor, "Steps toward the

Hum. Engng.
L.

56]

M.

Isolation of Tonal

Memory

as a

Mental Element,"

Lab. Tech. Rep., 21 (1938).

Tilson, The Tilson-Gretscb Musical Aptitude Test, Chicago, Publ. Sch. Music Dept.,

Fred Gretsch Mfg., 1941.
R. Farnsworth, "Data on the Tilson-Gretsch Test for Musical Aptitude," J.

P.

£j\

4 (194^): 99-102.

Musical.,

C. E. Seashore, D. Lewis, and

^8]

J.

G. Saetveit, Manual, Seashore Measures of Musical

Talents,

N.Y., Psychol. Corp., 19^6.

^9]

G. Saetveit, D. Lewis, and C. E. Seashore,

J.

Revision of the Seashore Measures of

Musical Talent, Iowa City, U. of Iowa Press, 1940. Several researchers have adapted portions

of the Seashore battery for their own, nonmusical purposes.

Pitch-Memory Selection Test," OSRD

"A Revision of
Base,
60]

New

F.

S.

the

F.

March, 1944;

Navy Pitch-Memory Test," Med.

London, Conn., 9

See A. Ford, "The
J.

Lab., U.S. Naval

Res.

UCDWR

D. Harris and D. Charney,

Submarine

(195^0): i-io.

Salisbury and H. R. Smith, "Prognosis of Sight-Singing Ability of

School Students,"
61]

Report,

J. Appl. Psjcbol., 13

Normal

(1929): 42^-439.

A. Wright, "The Correlation between Achievement and Capacity in Music,"

J. Educ. Res.,

IJ (1928): 5^0-^6.

62]

H. C. Manor, "A Study

63]

P.

R.

in Prognosis," J. Educ. Psychol., 41 (19^0):

Farnsworth, "Are Music Capacity Tests

More Important

31— jo.

than Intelligence

Tests in the Prediction of Several Types of Music Grades?" J. Appl. Psychol., ig (193^):

347-3^064]

C. H. Lawshe,

Jr.,

and

W.

Wood, "Membership

F.

in

Musical Organizations as a

Criterion of Talent," Amer. J. Psychol., 60 (1947): 2^0-25^3.
65^]

P.

J.

Fay and

W.

C. Middleton, "Relationship between Musical Talent and Preferences

for Different Types of Music," J. Educ. Psychol., 32 (1941):
66]

E. C. Dunlevy, "Musical Training and

J73-5'83.

Measured Musical Aptitude,"

J. Musical.,

4

(1944): i-^.
67] P. R. Farnsworth,

"The Relation of the Auditory Capacities to the Feeling of Being

Musical," J. Musical., 2 (1941):
68]
la.

69]

1

19-122.

H. M. Stanton, "Measurement of Musical Talent: The Eastman Experiment," U. of
Stud. Psjchol. Alusic, 2 (i93i^):

W.

S.

i

— 140.

Larson, "Practical Experience with Music Tests," Mus. Ed. J., 24 (1938): 31.

See also R. C. Larson, "Finding and Guiding Musical Talent," Mus. Ed.

22-2S.

J.,

42 {13 £s):

THE MEASUREMENT OF MUSICAL ABILITIES
M.

E.

70]

"A Study

Taylor,

in the Prognosis of Musical Talent," J. Exp. Ed., 10 (1941):

1-28.
71]

J.

McLeish, "The Validation of Seashore's Measures of Musical Talent by Factorial

Methods,"
72]

J.

Brit. J. Psychol., Stat. Sect., 3

(19^0): 129-140.

W. Dykema,

Kwalwasser and P.

Kwalwasser-Bjkema Music

Tests,

N.Y., Fischer,

1930.
73]

P. R. Farnsworth, "Studies in the Psychology of

Tone and Music,"

Genet. Psjchol.

Monog., IS (1934): Sect. 9.
74]

For descriptions of the

Chapter

K-D measures

of melodic taste and tonal

movement

see

7.

7S] G. M. Gilbert, "'Aptitude' and Training: A Suggested
K-D Music Test Norms, "y. Appl. Psjchol. 25 (1941): 326-330;
,

Restandardization of the
P. R. Farnsworth, op.

cit.

76] J. A. Holmes, "Increased Reliabilities, New Keys, and Norms for a Modified
Kwalwasser-Dykema Test of Musical Aptitudes," J. Genet. Psychol., 8s (i95'4): 65-73.
77]

M.

T. Whitley,

"A Comparison of

the Seashore and

Kwalwasser-Dykema Music

Tests," Teach. Coll. Rec, 33 (1932): 731-751.
78]

J.

79]

R.

Kwalwasser, Kwalwasser Music Talent

M. Drake, "Four New

Test,

N.Y., Mills Music, 1953.

Tests of Musical Talent," J. Appl. Psjchol., ij (1933):

136-147.
80]

R.

M. Drake, Drake

81]

H.

S.

Calif.

82]

Tests,

Chicago, Science Research Associates,

Whistler and L. P. Thorpe, Whistler-Thorpe Musical Aptitude

R.

W.

Los Angeles,

Lundin, "The Development and Validation of a Set of Musical Ability Tests,"

No. 10 (1949).

H. Wing, "Tests of Musical Ability and Appreciation,"

No. 27 (1948); "Some Applications of Test Results
Psychol., 24 (1954): 161-170.
84]

Test,

Test Bureau, 1950.

Psjchol. Monog., 63,

83]

Musical Aptitude

E. K. Strong, Jr.,

Stanford U. Press,
teachers, see

M.

Vocational Interest Blank for Men, Revised Form

1938.

Kleist, C.

Interest Scales for

Brit. J. Psjchol.

For

a further

Monog. Suppl.,

to Education in Music," Brit. J. Educ.

M, Stanford,

Calif.,

attempt to construct Strong scales for music

H. Rittenhouse, and

P. R. Farnsworth, "Strong Vocational

Music Teachers," Occupations, 28 (1949):

1

00-101.

2^3

CHAPTER TEN

Applications of Music to Therapy

and Industry

Increasingly

over the years, music

therapy for the emotionally
output.
already

ill

and

as a

is

as a

Indeed, the job of furnishing music to shops and stores has

become

a sizable

and lucrative business. Yet the student

asks for proof of the medical and industrial

he will recognize

little that

likely

being employed both

goad to increase industrial

as

worth of music

having scientific validity.

he will be fed anecdotes and legends,

is

who

offered

More

than

e.g., the Bible story of

the mentally disturbed King Saul listening to young David's harp
playing.

He may be shown

improvement

in

learns that the

data

which demonstrate unmistakable

work output or emotional

adjustment.

music was given along with

a host of

or changes in industrial atmosphere and he
part music

may have

is left

But then he

other therapies

puzzling over the

played in the process, wondering

if it

was

really

the music that induced the changes.
o

Physiological Changes
As

a preliminary step

toward getting

a

clearer picture of the

it may be well to
human physiology. The
and modify moods needs no further

potential role of music in therapy and industry

examine what
thesis that

is

music can

defense (p. 94).

2^4

known

of

its

elicit

power

to affect

But can tone and rhythm affect blood pressure,

APPLICATIONS OF MUSIC TO THERAPY AND INDUSTRY
pulse,

man's

respiration rate,

processes?

life

may be

It

and other physiological manifestations of

categorically stated that music can

markedly

affect the

bodily processes. Yet the effects are not so striking as was once
thought.

It

was believed at one time, for instance, that the heart would,

within limitations, accommodate

its

beat to the pulse of the music

being heard. By the use of better apparatus and controls, however,
it

has been possible to re-examine the

data.^

Of

problem and

the subjects so far studied, only one

collect additional

showed any tendency

for synchrony of heartbeat and musical pulse, and even here the

correspondence was so

slight that it

may have been a matter of chance.
combed the experimental

Diserens and Fine, after having carefully
literature

up through the middle 1930's

on physiological processes,
Music

.

.

.

increases

its

regularity

.

.

.

offer the following conclusions:

.

.

.

.

increases

or

de-

accelerates respiration and decreases

produces marked but variable effect on volume,
.

.

modes.

stimuli of different

With some

.

.

pulse and blood pressure

still

metabolism

bodily

creases muscular energy

for studies of musical effects

.

.

lowers the threshold for sensory

.influences the internal secretions.^

rephrasing the conclusions of Diserens and Fine can

be accepted.

It is

reasonably clear that music has on occasion

increased the bodily metabolism of a certain few people and has
affected their muscular energy, respiration, and/or circulation.

generalization in this area

matons reacting

in a

is

But

dangerous since humans are not auto-

pushbutton fashion to music.

Nor

is all

music

the same, and the effect of one composition does not necessarily

resemble that of another. The idea that music can lower thresholds
(raise acuities) in

other sensory areas

experimenters. 3

However,

this

Dannenbaum, who found the
significantly impaired,

is

held by contemporary Soviet

notion has been questioned by

visual acuity of

each of his subjects

not improved, by the presence of music. 4 But

APPLICATIONS OF MUSIC TO THERAPY AND INDUSTRY
whether or not music can improve

visual acuity as such,

it is

able,

according to Lovs^enstein,5 to restore the size of eye-pupils w^hich

have been experimentally fatigued by
thus reduced in

size.

It

many exposures

to light and

should be added, however, that Lowenstein

obtained this effect with "very musical people" only.

He

is,

therefore,

inclined to think that this "restitution phenomenon," as he calls

it, is

not

a function of tonal stimuli per se but rather of psychological stimuli,
i.e.,

of stimuli which have real musical significance to the listener. At

any rate, here

An

an example of the fatigue-reducing capacity of music.

unpublished study by Dreher^ showed that the unmusical

well as the

more musical

exhibit galvanic changes

7

the

unmusical subjects.

There was

direct

a

as

while listening to

much weaker

But these electrical changes were found to be

music.
in

is

correspondence

between the degree of galvanic change and the importance of music
in the life of the listener.

Thirty-six students, randomly selected by Ellis and Brighouse from

volunteers in undergraduate psychology,^ listened to three recordings



and

Hall's Blue Interval, Debussy's Prelude to the Afternoon of a Faun,

Liszt's

Hungarian Rhapsody No. 2

—while records were made of

their heart and respiration rates. Physiological records

were

also

kept

for several minutes before, and for as long as five minutes after, the

playing of the music. These experimenters report that no statistically
significant changes in heartbeat

in respiration rate

were found

were apparent

at

any time. But increases

in almost all subjects, particularly

during the playing of the Liszt and Hall numbers.
that

no respiration change

It is

of interest

lasted for longer than five minutes after

the cessation of the listening session, a finding which offers

encouragement to the music
It

therapists.

goes almost without saying that the findings of the Ellis-Brig-

house experiments might have been greatly different

if

either the

extremely musical or monotones had been tested, or

if

quite dis-

From what

is

known

similar compositions had

2^6

little

been presented.

of

APPLICATIONS OF MUSIC TO THERAPY AND INDUSTRY
we

psychology

should expect enormous individual differences to

And contemporary

exist in physiological responses to music.

ments bear out

experi-

In fact there are persons so readily

this expectation.

affected by music that listening to certain compositions leads to

And

epileptic attacks. 9

who

deaf,

can get

little

at the

other extreme there are the pitch-

or nothing from music except through the

kinesthetic and tactual senses.

The

on

physiological effects of music

the listener will be large or small, depending on the nature of the

composition which

A number

being heard.

is

of variables are of

importance here, including, among others, the presence or absence
of abrupt tempo changes, the acceptability or unacceptability of the

composition, and the personal associations the music has for the

As Miles and

listener.

Tilly have demonstrated,

the chief cause of respiratory changes.



change in tempo

While tempo change

a factor in altering circulation, the attitude of the listener

composition seems to be a

far

more important

principles hold both while the listener

As the music becomes more

tized.

is

alert

familiar,

is

is

also

toward the

variable.

and while he

is

These
hypno-

more "understood" and
become more marked.

appreciated, the physiological changes tend to

How
The

extensive, then,

answ^er

music

is

elicits,

is

music's

power

to affect

body processes?

not an easy one to make. For the physiological changes

while substantial and varied in certain musical persons,

are relatively insignificant in the unmusical and appear to be shortlasting at best.

By and

large, the effects are greater the

music has "meaning" for the

may

call

That

is,

a given

the

composition

forth one set of effects in a musical person and quite

different changes in
differ

listener.

more

from one time

one not musically inclined.
to another in the

Or

the effects

same person depending on the
on the

training the individual has received in the meantime,
tions he has picked up, and

from these

facts that

on

his changes in

no composition

may

mood.

would follow

It

will be found

associa-

which can be

guaranteed to produce identical or even nearly identical physiological

17

FSP

APPLICATIONS OF MUSIC TO THERAPY AND INDUSTRY
changes

among

members

the

One

of any sizable population.

likely to find any considerable degree of generality of

is

not

body change

except perhaps where the effects are elicited through the hearing of

most of us have known well since

national and church hymns, music

With music which

early childhood.

common

of the

less a part

is

heritage, the chances of securing identical effects are slighter.

The Present Status of Music Therapy
Since music can undeniably alter both
physiological processes of
for therapy."

It

has,

and the physically
then, are
partial

its

ill

many

we know,

in

it

moods and some basic
may have some potential

long been used to treat the mentally

both preliterate and civilized cultures." Why,

medical qualities not more easily demonstrated?

answer can be found

hospitals.

persons,

It is

in the

way therapy

is

employed
want

natural that the psychiatrists should

mental health to the point where their patients can be

Most mental

speedily returned to their homes.
fully

in

to

A

mental

improve

safely

and

hospitals are dread-

crowded, and many more emotionally disturbed persons are

usually waiting to enter than can be accepted.

Under such

pressures, a research

program on musical or any other

kind of therapy can be only incidental,
a

must be
a

at best.

"buckshot" policy must be followed. That
tried

on each patient

in the

hope

combination of treatments will effect

of therapies makes

it

impossible to

tell

On

the curative side,

is,

a variety of therapies

that

some one of them or

a cure.

But

which one

this multiplicity

is

mainly respon-

when the patient improves, if he does improve. As a matter of
fact, it may sometimes be that no one therapy by itself contributes
very much to the subsequent "cure." The active agent may be the
friendly attention the patient has been receiving, or there may
sible

perhaps be some dynamic personality change which would have

occurred even though no therapy
2^8

at all

had been attempted.

APPLICATIONS OF MUSIC TO THERAPY AND INDUSTRY
With

the situation so beclouded, the one clear fact

is

that definitive

statements about the medical value of music must await the establish-

ment

another can be put through

some

where

of research-oriented hospitals

years away.

And

until

But

paces.

its

it

this

comes, there

research-heaven

winnow

Music

in Physical

is still

one can do but

is little

examine the anecdotes which abound and the
accept and from them try to

one therapy and then

first

beliefs the specialists

at least a

few grains of

fact. ^3

Therapy

Boring indeed are the exercises that the muscle- and joint-injured

must practice day
become,
able.
fit

if

It is

But

after day.

not actually enjoyable,
relatively easy to find

the needs of each patient.

encouraged.
piano.

if

set

at least

to music,

the exercises

considerably

more endur-

music with tempos and rhythms to
For leg injuries dancing

Finger exercise needs

may

call for

is

often

the playing of the

and throat muscles need to be strengthened, the

If facial

playing of brass or

wood-wind instruments may be

the leg-injured patient

who

possesses

For

appropriate.

no musical or dancing

skill,

pumping of a small organ may be prescribed. Time seems to
more quickly when music accompanies the therapeutic exer-

the foot
pass

cises,

and patient morale tends to be better maintained. This type of

musical therapy

is

usually administered in hospitals but

reasonably beneficial in the home,
benefit derived

Music

in

from seeing others

it

be

although here the additional

also exercising

is

missing.

Mental Therapy

When

ESTABLISHING CONTACT WITH THE PATIENT.
patient has

will

withdrawn

into a

world of daydreams, an early step

to re-establish the patient's contact with the

therapy

is

reality.

Often the psychotic will not

talk or take

much

the
in

world of

interest in
25-9

17-2

APPLICATIONS OF MUSIC TO THERAPY AND INDUSTRY
his surroundings.

Yet the patient

who

earUer had a deep love of

music and no frustrations associated with
to

melody and rhythm when he

principle.
to

match

patient.

drawn,

The music

is

sometimes respond

as to

be isomoodic and isotempic,

mood

and "mental tempo" of the

chosen so

so far as possible the

Thus, quiet, unobtrusive music
lively

is

selected for the with-

music for the maniacal, "feminine" music for one of

feminine mentality,

and held,

will

In such cases, Altshuler suggests the use of his "iso"

stimulation.
^4

it

will not react favorably to verbal

etc.

Later, after the attention has been aroused

a gradual shift in

type of music

is

engineered in the hope

that the patient's mental and physiological states will also change.

PLEASANTNESS AND THE FEELING OF BEING RESTED. Many
more alert and rested after listening to our favorite compositions.
A somewhat similar situation exists among those in
mental hospitals. But among the hospitalized, listening to music has
of US feel

the additional advantage of helping to pass the time, which can drag
frightfully.

Research has shown that a

between the

fairly

feeling of restfulness and the pleasantness of

between tiredness and unpleasantness).
.

strong relationship exists

^5

music (or

During the course of

life,

become associated. We should keep the fact of
mind and not listen to strange, and so potentially

pleasantness and rest
this association in

unpleasant, music at a time

when we

to understand the unfamiliar

is

feel

mentally fatigued. Striving

not a restful undertaking.

MUSIC AS A SOCIALIZING AGENT. The psychotic can be
regarded as a person who is poor in his interpersonal adjustments. He
badly needs music or some other resocializing agent. To supply such
a need, unison group singing and dancing are encouraged in the more
therapeutically oriented of our mental hospitals. These activities take
a

minimum

of alertness. Yet they bring about vitally needed inter-

personal contacts, they break the

260

monotony of institutional life, and,

APPLICATIONS OF MUSIC TO THERAPY AND INDUSTRY
for a short time at least, they

may

patient.

singing, the patient

once again

is

dissipate the personal worries

While engaged

which have beset the

a

member

in dancing or

group

of a functioning group.

THE SENSE OF ACHIEVEMENT AND PRESTIGE. Music

IcSSOnS

are often given in mental hospitals so that the patient can feel that he
is

achieving a real

skill,

leaves the institution.

comes the opportunity
or with vocal

one that he can continue to enjoy

With

skill in

playing a musical instrument

to perform in the hospital's orchestra or band,

place in the chorus. Such ensemble

skill a

he

after

a sense of achievement,

work

brings

bestowes prestige on the performers, and

aids in the process of socialization.

OTHER ATTEMPTS TO USE MUSIC.

With

SO

many

physicians

and musicians convinced of the existence of large physiological

from music,
hospitals
active,

it

and

effects

was inevitable that music should have been used

clinics to quiet the apprehensive, to

in

calm the hyper-

to stimulate the depressed, to reduce accidents, ^^ and to

distract

those

about to undergo dental work or surgery.

The

reported successes of such uses of music vary with the enthusiasms
Sad to relate, therapy data are rarely recorded in

of the therapists.

terms which mean
perceived benefit

is

much

to

the scientist.

more properly

To what

extent the

attributable to the attention the

patient gets than to the music cannot at present be figured.

may be an appreciable
remains,

fraction of the therapeutic effect.

however, that there are patients

apprehensive,

less

who

are

The

But

it

fact still

visibly

less

maniacal, or less depressed after sessions with

musical therapy.

the"musicopoeia."

Although normal persons and psychotics

of like subculture react in a fairly similar fashion to music, there

seems to be enough dissimilarity to

justify the rule that all

music to
261

APPLICATIONS OF MUSIC TO THERAPY AND INDUSTRY
be employed with psychotics should be pretested on psycho tics /7
This rule appears to be followed rather religiously in

while in others the therapist's

own

some quarters

feelings or those of his friends

furnish the guide for his selection of the compositions he will use.
If

he

is

patients

psychoanalytically oriented, he

on simple

may

Each musical therapist seems to have

"^^

his favored list of musical

compositions. Thus, for relieving serious headaches

Rhapsody

No.

1,

Mendelssohn's

Prince Igor; "popular"

Suite

Paris;

in

Brown

offers

includin^r such old "classics" as Liszt's Hungarian

ones like Grofe's

and "moderns"

and Copland's Lincoln

Portrait.

like

Don

Mozart's

Elijah,

Offenbach's Tales of Hojfmann, Beethoven's

American

"seem

folk tunes, following the theory that these

to resupply or reactivate the mother-child complex.

some 40 pieces

convalescing

start his

Fidelio,

Mississippi

Giovanni,

and Borodin's

and Gershwin's

Khachaturian's Masquerade

^^

Arrington suggests the following "reassuring" compositions for use
just before electroshock treatment:

Beethoven's
First

Piano

/kfoonii^ Ac Sonata,

Concerto

Largo from Bach's Concerto

Brahms's Intermezzo

(Second

Movement),

in

E

Flat,

in A,

Chopin's

Mendelssohn's

Italian

Symphony (Second Movement), and Rachmaninov's Second Concerto

(Second Movement).

During the awakening period Arrington con-

siders these as appropriate: Kern's

Look Tonight,

Chopin's Waltzes

Album, and Adamson's Time on

Show Tunes, Fields's The Way You

in

A and

Mj

Hands.

C,

Berlin's Eddie Duchin

To Arrington

are musical tonics: Tchaikovsky's Sixth Symphony (Third

these eight

Movement),

Beethoven's Egmont Overture, Chopin's Prelude, Opus 28 No.
Hungarian Rhapsody No. 2, Bizet's

Marches, Offenbach's Gaiete

Toreador'' s Song,

Parisienne,

1, Liszt's

Sousa's Military

and Bach's Prelude and Fugue

in

E Minor; and these nine are musical sedatives: Mascagni's Cavalleria
Rusticana (Intermezzo), Schubert's Ave Maria, Saint-Saens's The Swan,

Brahms's LuUahj, Beethoven's
Chopin's Nocturne

262

in

G

Sixth

Symphony (Second Movement),

Minor, Debussy's Clair de Lune, Schubert's

APPLICATIONS OF MUSIC TO THERAPY AND INDUSTRY
Quartet

Sonata.

B

in

and

(Andante),

Minor

Flat

Beethoven's

Moonlight



Sugarman attempts to lower "emotional high blood pressure"
through renditions of a variety of compositions which include:
Bach's Concerto

Bruckner's Mass
of the

D

in

E

in

Minor for

Bartok's Sonata for Piano,

Sjmphony No.

3,

Dead, and Tchaikovsky's Swan Lake Ballet

among which

eating he suggests 20 pieces,
Violin,

Ravel's La

and

Rachmaninov's

Isle

To accompany

Suite.

are Bartok's Sonata for

Debussy's Children's Corner

Valse,

Concerto No. 2 in A,

To

Violin,

Minor, Ives's

Ives's Sonata No. 2 for Violin

Suite,

and

Liszt's

Piano.'^^

replace jealousy and suspicion with contentment, Hillard offers

Anthiel's

Quartet No. 5,

Bach's

4,

Chopin's Nocturne

and Ravel's Quartet

mentions

No.

Sonata

Piano

Bach's

in

F To
.

Italian

in

D

Cantata

Flat,

No.

Milhaud's

21,

Suite Frangaise,

relieve chronic hatred this

Concerto,

Haydn's

Bartok's

same author

Sjmphony,

Clock

and

Sibelius 's Finlandia.^'^

Girard

claims

to

reduce

Beethoven's Moonlight Sonata,

anger with

Bach's

Grofe's Aviation

Sonata in D, and Franck's Sjmphonj in

D

Minor.

Cantata

Suite,

To overcome

Girard would play Chopin's Mazurkas and Preludes,
Waltzes, Nevin's

The

lists

literature. ^4

Narcissus,

and Rubinstein's Melody

above are typical of what one

finds

in

f

in

No.

2,

Prokofiev's

the

anxiety,
Strauss

.^3

the current

Quite clearly they are the resultants of "rule of thumb"

rather than scientific procedures.

changes both in

mood and

It is

very probable, of course, that

in overt activity have

sometimes occurred

following the playing of these and other musical compositions, but
the causal connections between the music and the behavioral changes
are obscure.

Moreover,

at this stage in the history of the institu-

tional use of music, little or

venture to another

is

no generalization from one therapeutic

warranted. The reader should not conclude,

however, that music has no future

may demonstrate

in therapy.

Indeed, later research

large benefits attributable to the music alone.

At
263

APPLICATIONS OF MUSIC TO THERAPY AND INDUSTRY
the very least,

it

would seem

morale

that the

many

lift

patients

receive from this kind of treatment amply justifies the effort involved.

And

surely

The

no great harm

Effects

is

likely to

come from

of Music on Achievement

MUSIC AND ACTIVITY.

Although many of the phenomena of

the laboratory cannot be replicated in "real"
carry over or at least can be

may

facilitate later

work

made

sway of people

who were
life

many

life,

others

to yield cues and principles

v^ill

which

outside the laboratory. Thus, Husband's

finding, that several rather different sorts of

applicable to

the use of music.

attempting to stand

generally. ^5

music
still,

all

increased the

would seem

As might have been guessed,

caused greater sway than music of other

styles.

Work

at the

to

be

jazz

Stanford

University laboratories has carried the Husband study farther to

show

that even thinking of jazz

music can increase sway. That

is,

more while they
rhythmical music than when they imagined

subjects told to stand as quietly as possible swayed

imagined hearing strongly

themselves studying in an easy chair.

It

would seem,

then, that the

activation of either the ear or the "mind's ear" can lead to slight

body movements which are of measurable

size.^^

Diserens and Fine report a series of laboratory experiments on the

As pioneer work their research

influence of music on behavior.

should be commended.
the present day

it

However, viewed

inust be said that the study generalizes too freely

and concerns too few subjects.

show the

froin the vantage-point of

variety of areas

it

Its

conclusions are given here only to

encompassed.

Music tends to reduce or delay fatigue and consequently
increases muscular endurance. Music has no definite effect on
precision or accuracy of movement,
to the

rhythm of the work.

It

if

the rhythm

reduced accuracy

and handwriting, the result being shown

264

in

is

not adapted

in typewriting

an increased

number

APPLICATIONS OF MUSIC TO THERAPY AND INDUSTRY
of errors.
vs^riting

Music speeds up such voluntary

and handw^riting.

It

increases the extent of muscular reflexes

Music reduces normal

dravs^ing, etc.

activities as type-

also accelerates respiration.

employed

Music

in writing,

except in

suggestibility,

the case of direct suggestion involving color, in which case
suggestibility

produce

is

Music seems to have

increased.

a shift in

a

tendency to

normal preference for chromatic and achro-

matic impressions, the change being toward the blue end of the

spectrum and the white end of the achromatic

series.

Music

has a tendency to reduce the extent of illusion by acting as a
distracting factor.

of the

human body

Music influences the
as

electrical conductivity

manifested by increased fluctuations in the

psychogalvanic reflex. ^7

TYPING, MEMORY, AND READING.
effects of jazz

no

effect

errors.

and dirges on

on the speed of

is

his subjects' typing,

seemed to have

did increase their

Dirge

Jensen's findings appear reasonable.

obviously not in synchrony with good typing speed.

work accuracy would very
environment which,

likely

like jazz,

found by Whitely to have

insignificant,

compete

for attention.

might not have appeared

might be

fitting to

Music was

a very small detrimental influence

however, that with

And

be affected by factors of the

learning and retention of verbal material. ^9

It

jazz
it

Dirges, on the other hand, decreased the typing speed but

had no effect on errors.
time

Jcusen has Studied the

Although

typing. ^^

slightly

The

effects

on the

were

so

changed conditions they

at all.

describe here

some experiments

at

Stanford

University in which subjects were engaged in pursuit and codelearning tasks while in the presence of attention-getting noise

approximately the 70-decibel

level. 3°

After a

half of the subjects continued their tasks
in their ears while the other

number

with the noise

group proceeded

of

trials,

still

at,

one-

booming

in relative quiet.

It is

of interest that no consistent or significant differences appeared

26^

APPLICATIONS OF MUSIC TO THERAPY AND INDUSTRY
between the performances of these two groups during any of the
Here

trials.

organism

is

an example of the fact that

who

will

learn

and

man

a rather adaptable

is

under

retain

extremely

trying

circumstances.

Although learning can proceed reasonably well

one might think would be

in the face of

what

a considerable distraction, there are limits

beyond which the disturbance becomes

a real

detriment to learning.

Thus, Fendrick showed that music could be a serious distraction to
persons reading very difficult material. 3^
It

may be

that psychologists are minimizing the distracting eff^ects

of music through their almost exclusive use of college students as
subjects.

If

persons

fifty

years of age or

more were

to be tested, the

data might look quite different, for these older persons learned to

read and study without the blaring of radio or phonograph. For them

music
and

is

not an integral part of the reading process.

his colleagues

slightly the

had no

Henderson

demonstrated that popular music lessened only

paragraph comprehension of their college subjects and

harm vocabulary

did not

When

effect

learning at

and that

all,

on either of these aspects of reading

"classical"
skill,

music

they were

presenting data which perhaps relate solely to college populations. 3^

The

fact that

of music and
is

some of
still

these latter claimed to study without the "aid"

were not bothered by the music of the experiment

of interest but cannot be taken at face value.

atypical students are far

come from homes

more

So

have

noisy with music from phonograph or radio.

read in the sanctuary of a quiet study

modern

For even these

likely than are their elders to

is

To

not characteristic of the

youth.'

far the discussion has

centered on music

as a distraction.

It

might be expected, however, that readers can sometimes benefit

from background music, particularly

tomed
True
266

if

they are young and accus-

to the simultaneity of such auditory and visual stimulation.

to expectation, examples of actual gain in reading speed can be

APPLICATIONS OF MUSIC TO THERAPY AND INDUSTRY
seen in the data of Freeburne and Fleischer
sitions to college subjects as they

had

Hall, too,

a facilitating effect.

reading of some persons. 34
school group

studied. 33

made higher

may have

music

played jazz compo-

other sort of music

found music beneficial to the

Almost 60 per cent of
scores

on the

Nelson Silent Reading Test

on

effect

facilitating

a

his junior-high-

Mikol and Denny demonstrate

while hearing background music.
that

who
No

rotary

pursuit

performances. 35

The

data so far considered indicate that for

hearing of music during study hours has

and study
for
is

still

many persons the forced

little

or no effect on reading

For others there may be adverse consequences and

habits.

others there may be measurable benefit.

The type of music

that

heard, the difficulty of the material to be read, and the study and

reading habits of the person being tested appear to be the pertinent
variables

which account

for the diversity of effects so far reported.

REPETITIVE WORK.

The proof of music's

usefulness for in-

dustry has been best demonstrated for repetitive work. This
surprising

when

world over have

not

Negro stevedores and laborers the

recalled that

it is

is

work

typically synchronized their

speeds to the

tempos of music and have derived much benefit therefrom. Their
morale has been

lifted

smoother and more

Modern

and their work movements have been made

efficient

shops, too, have

by the directing force of group singing.

work which

for these factory workers that

them music

highly repetitive, and

it is

played.

For

in industry

number of possible benefits
smoother motor performance. 3^

has a

and make for

the daydreams which occupy
in this class of labor.
to hear

is

most music

much

has his interests at heart and
conditions. There

may

is

It

of the mental

Moreover, the

music while laboring may

It

:

may relieve fatigue
may supply food for

life

fact that the

signify to

him

is

of those engaged

worker

that the

is

allowed

management

attempting to improve his working

be, then, a considerable raising of morale.

267

APPLICATIONS OF MUSIC TO THERAPY AND INDUSTRY
NONREPETiTiVE WORK.
requiring

With

little intellectual effort

may prove

of music each day

siderable intellectual effort

is

nonrepctitive

the introduction of several periods

quite

worker

But where con-

worth while.

involved music

and may have a harmful effect on output.
feeling the

work

factory

It

is

may have

has for the music

less

often beneficial

should be noted that the

do with

little to

its

He may desire it and be quite certain that it is
work when in reality it is not. In those fewer instances,

value to his work. 37
aiding his

usually in offices,

who

where music

uses his brain

more

than his hands, the type of music found to be

appropriate resembles but
the labor

found to be of benefit to the worker

is

little

the kind used for

more manual. As might be

is

far softer, less regularly

workrooms where

guessed, office music must be

rhythmic, with fewer dynamic changes, and

without words.

OTHER CLAIMS FOR MUSIC.

Proponents of industrial music

have at one time or another maintained that, by the proper use of music
absenteeism and personnel turnover can be reduced; and physical
health, punctuality and plant safety can

students of plant music, however,

So

far,

have not found these

effects.

these alleged benefits have not been reported in the better

controlled studies. Yet

it

occasion. For might not
a

be improved. There are other

who

a

is

conceivable that they could occur on

worker be

company which brought music

become more punctual and

less

so filled with

to

its

good

will

toward

workers that he would

prone to "play" sick or

to quit his

job?

THE FORMULAE FOLLOWED.
in

complete agreement

The

in their musical prescriptions.

however, that care should be taken
beginning of recess periods. That
just before

268

authorities are

time to stop work,

by no means

They do

agree,

lest

music be conditioned to the

if

music were played each day

is,

its

presentation

would

give the

APPLICATIONS OF MUSIC TO THERAPY AND INDUSTRY
workers

home" cue which obviously would not

a "going

lead to

enthusiasm for work.

Some

students of industrial music take great pains to give the

workers exactly what they want to hear while others

much

preference aspect has been

overemphasized. These latter assert

that research attention should rather

But where preference

feel that the

be focused on output changes.

emphasized, folk music and operas are often

is

played to the foreign-born workers. The female voice

much

often than the male, since

less

appreciated.

The

British, as a

its

is

broadcast

higher register

matter of fact, rule out

all

is

not so

vocal music

because they have noticed a tendency for the laborer to stop work

and write down the

lyrics

he particularly enjoys. Sex differences for

the sort of music most preferred seem few in number.
differences

loom

larger,

Generation

with the older workers preferring, on the

whole, quieter and more serious music.
In

one extensive study of factory music, the optimum

when music

output was found to occur

time for the day

shift

Muzak Corporation,

i

2

effect

on

per cent of the

and go per cent for the night crew. 3^ The

the leading firm devoted to supplying music to

industry, has a rule of not

eight-hour workday.

played

v^^as

more than three hours

A common

formula

is

of music for each

to have ten to fifteen

minutes of music each half-hour in factories and ten to twenty
minutes each hour in those

offices

where music seems

to be favorably

received.

LIMITATIONS OF CONCLUSIONS. We have seen that the
made on behalf of industrial music largely await validation.

claims

Perhaps the most promising area

is

that of

worker morale,

factory workers playing an ever growing role in plant

worker morale
cerned.

With

so

is

for with

management,

becoming of increasing importance

to

all

con-

But even here definitive experiments have yet to be made.

many workers

calling for

music

in their factories,

it

is

269

APPLICATIONS OF MUSIC TO THERAPY AND INDUSTRY
probable that management will continue to provide for

this desire,

thus giving further opportunities for the gathering of sound data.
Eventually, then, a

more adequate assessment

may

of industrial music

be forthcoming. 39

Notes
D. M. Johnson and M. Trawick, "Influence of Rhythmic Sensory Stimuli upon the

i]

Heart-Rate,"

6 (1938): 303—310.

M. Diserens and H.

C.

2]

y. Psychol.,

A Psychology of Music, Cincinnati, pubUshed by the

Fine,

authors, 1939, p. 25^3.

D. London, "Research on Sensory Interaction

I.

3]

in the Soviet

Union,"

Psychol. Bull.,

51 (i9?4): Si^-S^^-

A. Dannenbaum, "The Effect of Music on Visual Acuity," Sarah Lawrence Stud., 4
(194-S)- 18-26.

4]

O.

^]

Lowenstein,

M. Grunewald

Der psychische

RestitutionseJJekt,

in Music Therapy, E. Podolsky, ed.,

Basel,

Schwabe,

See also

1937.

N.Y., Philosophical Library, 1954,

241-2^1.
R. E. Dreher, "The Relationship between Verbal Reports and Galvanic Skin Responses

6]

to Music," Thesis, Indiana U., 1947.

The

7]

galvanic skin response refers to the fact that the electrical resistance of the skin

measurably decreased whenever, during emotional

is

the skin surfaces; R.
j. Psychol.,
8]

D.

I.

S. Ellis

and G. Brighouse, "Effects of Music on Respiration and Heart-Rate," Amer.

6s (19^2): 39-47; also published
Philosophical Library, 195^4, 1^8-169.

M.

produced on

44 (1957): 111-127.

J. Psychol.,

9]

states, perspiration is

Henkin, "The Prediction of Behavior Response Patterns to Music,"

"Two

Critchley,

(1942): 182-184;

S.

in Music Therapj, E. Podolsky, ed.,

N.Y.,

Cases of Musicogenic Epilepsy," J. Royal Naval Med. Serv., 28

Taylor, "Musicogenic Epilepsy: Case," J. Kojal 'Naval Med. Serv.,

28 (1942): 394-39S.
10]

J.

Gazette,
1

1]

R. Miles and C. R. Tilly, "Some Physiological Reactions to Music," Guy's Hospital

4^ (193^): 319-322.

Music therapy

of the therapists
I

2]

F.

3]

now

denotes

a

very young but growing profession. The house-organ

the Bulletin of the National Association for Music Therapj.

Densmore, "Importance of Rhythm

American
1

is

That music therapy

is

in its infancy

270

Songs for the Treatment of the Sick by

can also be seen in the fact that the

course designed to create specialists in this
institution

in

Indians," Sci. Month., 79 (1954): 109-112.

which

started the curriculum

field

was not introduced

was Michigan

first

until

State University.

four-year

1944.

The

APPLICATIONS OF MUSIC TO THERAPY AND INDUSTRY
14]

M.

I.

Altshuler, "Rational Music-Therapy of the Mentally

Assoc. Proc,

i^]

W.

C. Middleton,

et al.,

H.

S.

Mus.

Nat.

Teach.

"The Effect of Music on Feelings of Restfulness-Tiredness,

and Pleasantness-Unpleasantness,"
16]

111,"

1939, 15^3-15^7.

J. Psjchol.,

IJ (1944): 299—318.

Whiting, in "Effect of Music on Hospital Accident Rate," Amer.

J. Ment. T)ef.,

SI (1947): 397—400, maintains that music played over the radio in a hospital for mental
defectives can produce a drastic reduction in the

number

of accidents on the wards.

For

further material on music in hospitals for mental defectives see A. Wendelin and T. L.

Engle, "A Survey of Musical Activities in Institutions for the Mentally Deficient," Amer.
J. Ment. Dejic,

17]

B. Simon,

S9

{^9S4-)'-

et al.,

206-209.

"The Recognition and Acceptance of

Patients, "J. Nerv. Ment. Dis.,
18]

G.

W.

Mood

in

Music by Psychotic

II4 (1951): 66-78.

Ainlay, "The Place of Music in Military Hospitals," Etude, 63 (1945^): 433,

468, 480.
19]

M. Brown

L.

in Music Therapj, E. Podolsky, ed.,

N.Y., Philosophical Library, 19^4,

13^-138.
20]

G. E. Arrington,

I9j'4,

2^2-287. See

Jr., in

Music Therapy, E. Podolsky, ed., N.Y., Philosophical Library,

also the longer lists

from the chapter by H. G.

Price,

et al.,

in the

same book, pp. 95^—100.
Sugarman

in Music Therapy, E. Podolsky, ed.,

N.Y., Philosophical Library, 19^4,

21]

P.

22]

B. Hillard in Music Therapy, E. Podolsky, ed., N.Y., Philosophical Library,

I

1954,

21-1 29.

23]

J.

Girard in Music Therapy, E. Podolsky, ed., N.Y., Philosophical Library,

195^4,

loi— 106.
24]

For further references on music therapy, see L. Gilman and

Your Emotions, N.Y., Liveright, 19^2, 24-^^;

S.

H. Licht, Music

F. Paperte,

in Medicine,

Music and

Boston,

New

England Conservatory of Music, 1946; D. M. Schullian and M. Schoen, eds., Music and
Medicine, N.Y., Schuman, 1948; D. Soibelman, Therapeutic and Industrial Uses of Music,
N.Y., Columbia U. Press, 1948;

and Therapy," Mus. Teach. Nat.
25]

R.

W.

de Wall, "Functional Use of Music in Industry

Proc, 1944, 147-IJ3.

Husband, "The Effects of Musical Rhythms and Pure Rhythms on Bodily

Sway," J. Gen.
26]

W. Van

Ass.

Psychol., 11

(1934): 328-336.

That music can stimulate compensatory movements which can aid one's sense of

balance has been demonstrated by

W.

Lavere in "The Influence of Musical Training and

Musical Accompaniment on the Sense of Equilibrium," Master's Thesis, Syracuse U., 19^0.
27]

C.

M. Diserens and H.

Fine,

A Psychology of Music, Cincinnati, published by the

authors, 1939, pp. 273-274.
28]

M.

B. Jensen,

"The Influence of Jazz and Dirge Music upon Speed and Accuracy of

Typing," J. Educ. Psychol., 22 (1931): 4^8-462.

271

APPLICATIONS OF MUSIC TO THERAPY AND INDUSTRY
29]

"The Influence of Music on Memory,"

P. L. Whitely,

J. Gen. Psychol.,

10 (1934):

137-15130]

P. R. Farnsworth,

J. Genet. Psychol.,

31]

P. Fendrick,

Res., 31

32]

M.

traction
33]

"Continued Training with the Omission of Certain Nebenreize,"

SO (1937): 277-282.

"The Influence of Music Distraction upon Reading Efficiency,"

J. Educ.

(1937): 264-271.
T. Henderson, A. Crews, and

on Reading

J.

Barlow, "A Study of the Effect of Music Dis-

Efficiency," J. Appl. PsjchoL, 29 (1945):

313-317.

C. M. Freeburne and M. Fleischer, "The Effect of Music Distraction upon Reading

Rate and Comprehension,"

J. Educ. Psychol.,

43 (19^2): loi — 109.

"The Effect of Background Music on the Reading Comprehension of 278
34]
8th and 9th Grade Students," J. Educ. Res., 45 (19^2): 4(^1-45^8.
J. C. Hall,

l^]

B.

Mikol and M. R. Denny, "The Effect of Music and Rhythm on Rotary Pursuit

Performance," Percep. Mot.
36]

Skills,

According to E. Podolsky

1945^,

5 {i9Ss)'- 3-6.

as

reported in Music for Your Health, N.Y., Ackerman,

almost any musical rhythm can be employed in factories since the rhythm of the

worker's task

is little

affected by the

claim which needs verification.

rhythm of the music he

hears. This

is

an astonishing

See also "Soap Wrappers' Jig," J. Amer. Med. Assoc,

1

SJ

(i9SS)- 1329-133037]

W. McGehee

Psychol., 2

38]

and

J.

E.

Gardner, "Music in a Complex Industrial Job," Personnel

(1949): 405-417-

H. C. Smith, "Music in Relation to Employee Attitudes, Piecework Production, and

Industrial Accidents," Appl. Psychol. Monog.,
39]

No. 14 (1947).

For a most optimistic report of British industrial music see L. Kaplan and R. Nettel,

"Music in Industry,"

Biol.

Hum.

Ajfairs,

London, 13 (1948): 129— 13

j.

For a discussion of

Muzak in getting music to industry see E. M. Werner, Work Music by
Muzak, N.Y., Muzak Corp., 1948. See also R. L. Cardinell, "Music in Industry," in
Music and Medicine, D. M. Schullian and M. Schoen, eds., N.Y., Schuman, 1948, 3^2-366.
the part played by

272

Epilog ue

IVl usic has been variously

called the

most mathematical of the

the purest, and the least universal. That

it is

highly mathematical

have already seen, particularly in the data of Chapter
in the book, how^ever, has there

arts,

In

2.

been the suggestion that

we

no place

a satisfactory

mathematical formula for forecasting musical taste or beauty has been
or

likely to

is

be discovered. Since there

is

no

invariant relationship

between musical stimuli and human responses,
toward the same stimuli change with the

since

attitudes

years, such a formula

be unthinkable. The mathematics of the Lydian

mode

would

has not been

altered over the years since Plato banned this arrangement of tones
as

harmful to man's character.

porary Western world

this

But to many people of the contem-

sequence has become the only "proper"

scale.

Music
nature.

is

pure in the sense that

Music

each listener

is

tells

it

only rarely copies the sounds of

no clear-cut story with universal meaning. Yet

trained to read local meanings into

what he

He recognizes a Wagnerian motive, a needed resolution,
hymn and thus sharesin the enjoyment of his associates.
ever, the very paucity of these local meanings

opportunity to implant in the music his
associations.

fantasy

Thus

this art

medium

own

which

hears.

or a church
It is,

gives

how-

him the

personal images and

serves as an important aid to his

life.

273
18

FSP

EPILOGUE
The

acceptability of any musical

Listener satiations gradually build

Sometimes the changes are

form grows, but

later diminishes.

up and eventually

lead to change.

in the direction of innovations, while at

other times they point back toward what was acceptable at an earlier
time.

If

the

new forms

differ too

achieve general acceptance.
rule and

A

apt at first to

is

major

must look

thesis

for

its

this

explanations far
It is

change

L at every

seem undesirable

developed in

to physical science.

markedly from the old they do not

book

more

a violation of

is

some

to the conservative.

the notion that music

is

often to social science than

granted that the very beginnings of music

are tied to man's physical surroundings, to the presence of instrument-

building materials,

and to the physics of simple

ratios.

Man's

psychophysiological potentialities also affect music's development.

But the

fact that the

music of one culture has gone through certain

phases that are not duplicated in another culture must be explained
largely

on the

basis of sociopsychological

and historical factors, not

Music's changes, like style changes

physical or physiological factors.

generally, are lawful, not in an absolutistic or metaphysical sense,

but in the

way

that other social

phenomena

This book has attempted to uncover a

are lawful.

number

psychological variables behind music change.
to

come many other

factors will

be

isolated.

described here should lead to greater, not
for

it

seems to be

a

fairly

274

it.

less,

doubt

in the years

Research of the sort
musical enjoyment,

general principle that the

understands the complexities of a
takes in

No

of the socio-

phenomenon

the

more

more man
delight he

APPENDIX

I

1 N Chapter 6

it

The Musical Taste
American

.

was noted that

of an
Elite'

1938 the members of the American

in

Musicological Society had been sent questionnaires through which

was hoped
in

a

1944 and

measure of their musical
still

later in 195^1 the

taste

might be obtained. Again

members

were interrogated. At the time of the

it

last

of this elite organization
data-gathering, the 37^

who

cooperated were given two questionnaires, one containing 11 £
names of composers born before 1870 and the other listing 249

born since 1870. There were two

tasks.

The

was for the

first

musicologist-respondent to check in each of the two questionnaires
the ten musicians he felt had

composed music most

vs^orthy to

be

called to the attention of his children and his lay contemporaries.

As

a

second task he was to consider

all

474 names and, with the same

criterion in mind, was to choose the top

The

first

of the tables below gives the £o

frequently chosen

among

second table gives the
lists.

Note

25".

i

o

i

the

who were

composers born since

selected

the most

1870.

most often from the combined

that 24 of the first table appear also in the second.

a finding gives

The

no support to the notion so commonly held

Such

that the

musically elite honor neither their contemporaries nor those recently
deceased.
i]

See P. R. Farnsworth, "The Musical Taste of an American Elite," Hinricbsen's Musical

Year Book, J (i^ ^2):

18-2

ii2— ii6.

THE MUSICAL TASTE OF AN AMERICAN ELITE

Composers Born Since
Rank Order

1870

Rank Order

29-5

Barber, S.

4S-S

Malipiero, G.

2

Bartok, B.

36

Martinu, B.

49

Bax, A.

20

Menotti, G.

12

Berg, A.

13

Milhaud, D.

43

Berlin,

I.

23

Piston,

11

Bloch, E.

27

Poulenc, F.

9
40

Britten, B.

6

Chavez, C.

10

Rachmaninov,

Copland, A.

4
24

M.
Reger, M.

25-5

Respighi, O.

8

32

Dohnanyi, E.

16

Falla,

14

Gershwin, G.

4S-5

Grainger, P.

M. de

4S-S
5

W.

Prokofiev, S.

Romberg,

S.

Schonberg, A.

W.

29-5

Griffes, C.

49

Schuman,

29-5

Hanson, H.

21

Scriabin, A.
Sessions, R.

2S-S
3

33-S

S.

Ravel,

Harris, R.

29-5

Hindemith, P.

15

Shostakovich, D.

Hoist, G.

49

Sowerby, L.

18

Honegger, A.

40

Ibert, J.

1

Stravinsky,

I.

45-5

Thompson, R.

42

Varese, E.

17

Ives,

37

Khachaturian, A.

33-5

Kodaly, Z.

19

40

Kreisler, F.

22

Walton,

35

Krenek, E.

38

Webern, A.

C.

7

Vaughan Williams,
Villa-Lobos, H.

W.

Eminent Composers of all Time
Rank Order

nk Order

88-5

Bach,

J.

2

Bach,

J. S.

59-5

Britten, B.

41

Bruckner, A.

71

Bach, K. P. E.

52

Buxtehude, D.

16

Bartok, B.

27

Byrd,

1

Beethoven, L.

72

Berg, A.

21

Berlioz,

55-5

Bizet,

66

Bloch, E.

3

276

C.

H.

G.

Brahms,

J.

100

W.

Chausson, E.

11

Chopin,

47
43
40

Copland, A.

6-S

F.

Corelli, A.

Couperin the Great
Debussy, C.

R,

THE MUSICAL TASTE OF AN AMERICAN ELITE
Rank Order

Rank Order

92

Donizetti, G.

10

Palestrina,

82-5

Dowland,

80

Pergolesi, G.

36

Dufay, G.

78

Dunstable,

38

100

J.

G.

74

Perotinus

88-5

Piston,

Dvorak, A.

34

Prokofiev, S.

Elgar, E.

45-5

Puccini, G.

76

Falla,

61-5

J.

M. de

W.

H.

20

Purcell,

Faure, G.

55-5

Rachmaninov,

97

Foster, S.

49

Rameau,

28-S

Franck, C.

26

Ravel,

S.

J.

M.

53-5

Frescobaldi, G.

85-5

Respighi, O.

45-5

Gabrieli, G.

44

Rimsky-Korsakov, N.

58

Gershwin, G.

59-5

Rossini, G.

85-5

Gesualdo, D.

74

Saint-Saens, C.

66

Gibbons, O.

69

Scarlatti,

A.

28-5

Gluck, C.

35

Scarlatti,

D.

84

Gounod, C.

22

Schonberg, A.

66

Gregory the Great

48

Grieg, E.

12

Schumann, R.

8

Handel, G.

31

Schutz, H.

100

Hanson, H.

94-5

Scriabin, A.

Haydn,

57

Shostakovich, D.

4

F. J.

6-5

Schubert, F.

23

Hindemith, P.

30

Sibelius, J.

80

Honegger, A.

82-5

Smetana, B.

77

Ives, C.

97

Sousa, J

33

Josquin des Pres

51

Strauss, J. Jr.

88-5

Landino, F.

18

Strauss, R.

25

Lasso,

13

Stravinsky,

32

Liszt, F.

92

Sullivan,

O.

74

Lully, J.

66

MacDowell,

42
38

I.

A.

94-5

Sweelinck,

19

Tchaikovsky, P.

Machaut, G.

92

Telemann, G.

Mahler, G.

50

Vaughan Williams, R,

97

Massenet,

14

Verdi, G.

17

Mendelssohn,

61-5

Victoria, T.

80

Menotti, G.

88-5

Villa-Lobos, H.

70

Milhaud, D.

38

Vivaldi, A.

15

Monteverdi, C.

24

Moussorgsky, M.

5

66

Mozart,

E.

J.

W.

Ockeghem,

F.

J.

53-5

Wagner, R.
Weber, C. M. von

63

Wolf, H.

9

J.

277

APPENDIX

2.

Glossary'

Skill

ability.

of any sort; relative importance of inheritance and

environment not considered

(cf. capacity, talent).

Ability to locate a pitch without the need

absolute or positive pitch.

of a reference tone; the allowable error
less

Signs indicating sharps,

accidental signs.
flats,

very slight, perhaps

flats,

double sharps, double

naturals.

arpeggio.

Tones of

atonality.

Absence of key

Throbbing

beats.

is

than lo cents.

a

chord played
in

in rapid succession.

music.

effect elicited

when two

tones very close together

in pitch are simultaneously sounded.
beta learning.

Negative practice, that

is,

the errors are deliberately

practiced.
cadence.

Melodic or harmonic figure which has come to have an

association with the ending of a phrase, a section, or

compo-

sition.

capacity.
cent.

* This

Basic potentiality; importance of heredity

Tonal span of

stressed.

200 of an octave.

glossary attempts to define psychological terms for the musician and musical terms

for the psychologist.

It is

of his craft described in

278

i/i

is

to be expected that the psychologist will prefer to see the terms

more

exact language, as will the musician

his.

GLOSSARY
chromesthesia

Visual image of hallucinatory intensity aroused by

.

some auditory stimulus
coefficient

(cf. synesthesia).

Measure of correspondence between two

of correlation.

sets

ofmeasurements; values vary from i-oo (perfect correspondence)
through zero to



i-oo (completely inverse relationship).

Logarithmic unit of intensity so chosen

decibel.

as to

be equal, under

certain conditions, to one just-noticeable difference in loudness.

Tone sometimes

difference tone.

pitch are

in

elicited

simultaneously

when two

sounded;

its

tones separated

frequency

is

the

difference of the frequencies of the other two.
double stopping

Fingering two strings of a bowed instrument at once

.

double vibration (d.v.).
a sound-giving

A

drone.

Frequency (number of cycles) with w^hich

body

is

vibrating.

tone held for the duration of a melody or

at least for a

considerable period of time.
as

though

who attends more to external events and
own attitudes and mental processes.

objects

Imagery so intense that the person behaves

eidetic imagerj.

he were directly perceiving the music.

A

extrovert.

person

than to his
factor analysis.

Method of

resolving a set of interrelated variables

or tests into a few "factors" which are regarded as being
the fundamental variables underlying the original complex of
variables.

Span of

fifth.

fourth.

7

semitones.

Span of g semitones.

fugalform.

A

round; each

galvanic skin response.

new

Change

voice chases the preceding one.

in the electrical resistance of the skin

whenever, during emotional

states, perspiration

is

produced on

the skin surfaces.

279

GLOSSARY
Change

goal gradient.
harmonics.

with distance to a goal.

Cf. overtones.

Music

homophonj.
idiot

in degree of motivation

in

which the voices move

who

Person of very low i.q.

savant.

in step, e.g.,

has

hymn.

above average

achievement in some specialized area.
Pitch span between

interval.

two notes played simultaneously or

successively.

augmented.

Perfect or major increased by a semitone.

diminished.

Perfect or minor decreased by a semitone.

Spans of

major.

semitones (second, third, sixth, or

2, 4, 9,

or

i, 3, 8,

or 10 semitones (second, third, sixth, or

1 1

seventh).

Spans of

minor.

seventh).

Span separating two melodies which are identical but

parallel.

in different registers.
perfect.

Spans of

g, 7,

or 12 semitones (fourth,

Person preoccupied with his

introvert.

own

fifth,

attitudes

or octave).

and mental

processes.
principle.

iso

Notion that

should match the

a patient's

mood

mood

and "mental tempo"

and tempo of the music.

Family of tones held together by their relation to a tonic from

kej.

which the key

named.

Lowest and principal note of

key-note.

leading tone.

up

is

a scale; the tonic.

Major seventh or subtonic, so-called because

it

leads

to the tonic.

leger lines.

Additional short lines added above or below the

notes than cannot be

accommodated on the

staff for

staff.

major.
chord.

Three simultaneously or successively played notes com-

prising intervals of a major third plus a

280

minor

third.

GLOSSARY
Cf. interval.

interval.

Cf. mode.

mode.

Span of

second.

2

semitones,

Span of 9 semitones.

sixth.

Span of

seventh.

1 1

semitones.

Span of 4 semitones.

third.

Concentration of time devoted to learning with

massed practice.

between successive practice

little interval

Series of successively

melody.

sounded tones

sessions.

felt to

possess internal

organization.
Scale step smaller than a semitone.

microtone.

minor.

Three simultaneously or successively played notes com-

chord.

prising intervals of a

minor third

Cf. mode,

mode.

Span of

second.

Span of

sixth.

Span of

third.

8

i

semitone.

semitones.

Span of 10 semitones.

seventh.

3

semitones.

Forerunner of the key;

mode.

plus a major third.

Cf. interval,

interval.

differs in that the

mode

possible

arrangements of scale steps,

semitone

steps, or 2, 1,2, 2, 2, 1,2 steps, etc.;

e.g.,

2, 2,

now

has several
1,2,

2,

2,1

only two:

major and minor.
modulation.

Transition of a melody from one key up or

down

to

another key.
monotone.

Person so weak in pitch sensitivity that he cannot

recognize or carry a tune.
noise.

octave.

Complex

of sounds in

which no

definite pitch can

be detected.

Span of 12 semitones.
281

GLOSSARY
Theme with

organum.

moving
overlearning

parallel to

other voices a fourth below or a

material can for the

Tone

overtone.

elicited

practice
first

beyond

the

trial

where

the

time be reproduced correctly.

by the vibration of some fraction of the

major vibrating body,

e.g., 1/2, 1/3, 1/4, etc.

Cf. overtone,

partial.

Unit of loudness in which the value

phon.

above

it.

Continuing

.

fifth

of decibels a tone of 1000 d.v.

when judged

is

is

equal to the

number

above the reference intensity

equal in loudness to the tone in question.

Applied to music in which several melodies are played

polyphony.

simultaneously.
poljtonality

Presence of several simultaneous keys in a musical

.

composition.

The carrying on of the tone from note

portamento.

gaps (voice and
Pragnanz, law

of.

bowed

to note without

instruments); half-staccato (piano).

Persons tend to perceive objects in the simplest

arrangement possible.
Pitch level.

register.

reliability.
is

Degree of self-consistency; extent to which the measure

uninfluenced by factors intrinsic to or associated with

retroactive inhibition.

something very

Impairment of learning by the

later learning of

similar.

Type of correlation

rho.

it.

coefficient obtained through the handling

of rank differences,
rhythm
objective.

polyrhjthm.
subjective.

Periodicity with one element regularly emphasized.

Complex of several simultaneously played rhythms.
Rhythm read into sheer periodicity or into weak

objective rhythm.

282

GLOSSARY
Series of tones arranged in order of pitch and

scale.

employed

as

the

accepted notes of some system.

The twelve-semitone

chromatic.

Seven-note scale in the

diatonic.

scale of

white and black notes.

major or minor mode.

Where chromatic steps are all equal
Where the ratios use only the primes

equally tempered.
just-intoned.

i

Compromise

mean-tone.

scale

some

with

,

in ratio.
3

and

^.

which

tempering

allowed for a degree of modulation,
Pythagorean.
whole-tone.

Where the
Where each

ratios use only the

step

is

primes

i

whole tone from

a

and
its

3.

nearest

neighbor.
score.

Sum

mean.

of

all

the scores divided by the

number

of cases.

Middle value.

median.

Most frequently occurring

modal.

score.

sensations.

Those

kinesthetic.

arising

from the stimulation of receptors

in

muscles, tendons, and joints.

Those

organic.

arising

from the stimulation of receptors

in the

internal organs.

Degree to which one can distinguish stimuli which

sensitivity.

very
sonance.

differ

slightly.

Qualitative

effects

due

to

progressive

("horizontal")

changes and fusions, e.g., vibrato.
sostenuto

pedal.

Pedal found mainly on American and Canadian

pianos which maintains raised dampers.
stroboscope.

Instrument for observing the successive phases of

a

periodic motion by means of a light periodically interrupted.
Cf. tonoscope.
syncopation.

Placing an accent

where there would normally be no

accent.

283

G

LOSSARY
Image of hallucinatory intensity in one sensory area

synesthesia.

aroused by

a stimulus

from some other sense modality.

Cf.

chromesthesia

Cf. true beat.

takt.

talent.

Usually taken to

tempo.

Rate of speed

mean high capability;

at

which

allegro.

Lively tempo.

andante.

Slow tempo.

heredity

a musical passage

is

emphasized.

moves.

Quick tempo.

presto.
tests.

Tests taken to measure

achievement.

what has been learned.

Tests used to forecast whether or not training in a

aptitude.

particular area will be profitable.

Inverse of sensitivity (as used in this book).

threshold.

Effect

timbre.

due to the constellation of

partial

tones present;

"vertical" quality.

Tones

tone clusters.
fist, flat

tone symbol.

elicited

by depressing the piano keys with the

of the hand, or with the forearm.

Tonal ratio with powers of

2

extracted.

Cf. key-note.

tonic.

Stroboscope which gives a visual picture of a vocal or

tonoscope.

instrumental tone.
tremolo.

Vibrato with abnormally wide pitch span; also used to

describe a rhythmic unit with three unaccented elements.

Rapid oscillation of two tones which are perceived

trill.

as

two

tones.
tritone.

true beat.

Span of

six semitones.

Pulsations

which underlie phrase rhythms irrespective of

time signatures or number of notes

284

in the phrase.

GLOSSARY
validity.

Extent to which a

test

is

measuring what

it

claims to be

measuring.
vibrato.

Periodic oscillations of the vocal or instrumental tone in

pitch, intensity, and
6' ^

sometimes

in quality; rate

is

approximately

per second.

whole learning.

Material to be learned

ning to end,

i.e., is

not broken

is

down

gone through from begininto parts

which are

to

be

learned separately.

28s

APPENDIX

3.

Key

to Reference Abbreviations

Acta Oto-laryngologica , Stockholm

Acta Oto-laryng

Acta Psychologica

Acta Psychol.

American Imago

Amer. Imago

American Journal of Mental Dejiciencj

Amer. J. Ment. Defic.

American Journal of Psjchiatrj

Amer. J. Psychiat.

American Journal of Psychology

Amer. J. Psychol.

American Journal of Sociology

Amer. J. Sociol.

American Psychologist

Amer. Psychol.

American Sociological Review

Amer. Sociol. Rev.

Annals of the

New

York Academy of Sciences

Annals, N.Y. Acad. Sc.

Applied Psychology Monographs

Appl. Psychol. Monog.

Archiv fur die Gesamte Phonetik

Arch. Ges. Phonet.

Archiv Jur die Gesamte Psychologic

Arch. Ges. Psychol.

Archiv Jur Musikforschung

Arch. Musikforsch.

Archiv Jiir Rassen- und Gesellschajts-

Arch. Rass.-

Biologie, Einschliesslich Rassen-

u.

Ges. -Biol.

und

Gesellschajts-Hygiene

Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry

Arch. Neur. and Psychiat.

Archives of Otolaryngology, Chicago

Arch. Otolarjngol.

Archives of Psychology

Arch. Psychol.

Australian Journal of Psychology

Austral. J. Psychol.

Beihefte zur Zeitschrijt Jiir Experi-

Beihefte z. Zsch.J.

mentelle und Angewandte Psychologic

286

Psychol.

Ang.

KEY TO REFERENCE ABBREVIATIONS
Bell Laboratories Record

Biology and
British

Human

Affairs,

Bell Lab. Rec.

London

Journal of Educational

Hum.

Biol.

Ajfairs

Educ. Psychol.

Brit. J.

Psychology
British

Journal of Psychology

British Journal

of Psychology, General

Brit. J. Psychol.

Brit. J. Psychol.,

Gen. Sec.

Brit. J. Psychol.,

Monog.

Section
British

Journal of Psychology

Monograph Supplement
British Journal oj Psychology, Statis-

Suppl.
Psychol., Stat. Sec.

Brit. J.

tical Section

Math. Biophysics

Bulletin of Mathematical Biophysics

Bull.

Character and Personality

Char, and Person.

Child Development

Child Devel.

Child Development Monographs

Child Devel. Monog.

Deutsche Zeitschrift Jiir Nervenheil-

Dtsch. Zsch.J. Nervenhk.

kunde
Dioptric Review and British Journal of

Diopt. Rev. and Brit. J.
Physiol. Opt.

Physiological Optics

Genetic Psychology

Monographs

Genet. Psychol.

Harvard Psychological Studies

Harvard

Human
Human

Biology

Human

Engineering Lahoratorj

Hum. Engng.

Technical Report

Indiana University Publications,

Humanity
Institute

Psychol. Stud.

Biol.

Lab., Tech.

Rep.

Indiana U. Publ., Humanity

Series

of Radio Engineers, Proceedings

Monog.

Ser.
Inst.

Radio Eng., Proc.

International Congress of Musicology

Int.

Congr. Musicol.

Internationale Zeitschrift Jiir Individual-

Int.

Zsch.J. Indiv. -Psychol.

Psjchologie

Journal of Abnormal and Social

J.

Abn. Soc. Psychol.

Psychology

287

KEY TO REFERENCE ABBREVIATIONS
Journal of the Acoustical Society of

J. Acoust. Soc.

Amer.

America
journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism

J. Aesth.

Journal oj the American Medical

J. Amer.

Med.

Assoc.

Association

journal of Applied Psychology

J. Appl. Psychol.

Journal of Clinical Psychology

J. Clin. Psychol.

Com p.

Journal of Comparative Psychology

J.

Journal of Educational Psychology

J. Educ. Psychol.

Journal of Educational Research

J. Educ. Res.

Journal of Experimental Education

J. Exp. Ed.

Journal

erf

Experimental Psychology

Journal of the Franklin

Psychol.

J. Exp. Psychol.
J. Franklin Inst.

Institute

Journal of General Psychology

J. Gen. Psychol.

Journal of Genetic Psychology

J. Genet. Psychol.

Journal of Gerontology

J. Gerontol.

Journal of Heredity

J. Hered.

Journal of Musicology

J. Musi col.

Journal of Nervous and Mental

J. Nerv.

Ment. Dis.

Disease

Journal of Psjchology

J. Psychol.

Journal de Psychologic Normale

et

J. Psychol.

Norm. Path.

Pathologique

Journal of the Royal Naval Medical

J. Royal

Nav. Med. Serv.

Service

Journal of Social Psychology

J. Soc. Psychol.

Kwartalnik Psychologiczny

Kwart. Psjchol.

Medical Research Laboratory

,

U.S.

Med.

Res. Lab.

Naval Submarine Base, New London

Monographs of
in

the Society Jor Research

Child Development

Music Educators fournal

288

Monog.

Soc. Res. Child

Devel.

Mus. Ed. J.

KEY TO REFERENCE ABBREVIATIONS
Music Educators^ National Conference,

Mus. Educ. Nat. Conf. Yearb.

Yearbook

Music Supervisors Journal

Mus. Superv. J.

Music Supervisors National Conference,

Mus. Superv. Nat. Conf.

Yearbook

Yearb.

Music Teachers National Association,
Proceedings

Proc.

Musical Quarterly
Office

of

Mus. Teach. Nat. Assoc.

Music. Quart.

Scientific Research

and

OSRD

Report

Development, Report

Veabodj Bulletin

Peabody Bull.

Research Studies in Music, Peabody

Peabody Cons. Mus. Res.

Conservatorj oj Music

Stud.

Pedagogical Seminary

Ped. Sem.

Perceptual and Motor Skills

Percep. Mot. Skills

Personnel Psychology

Personnel Psychol.

Philologisch-historische Klasse,

Phil. -hist. Klasse Sitzungs-

Sitzungsberichte

berichte

Pillsbury Foundation Studies

Pillsbury Found. Stud.

Popular Science Monthly

Pop. Sci. Month.

Proceedings of the Iowa Academy of Science
Psychiatric, Neurologic,

and Medizinische

Psychologic, Leipzig

Proc. la. Acad. Sci.
Psychiat. Neurol.

Med.

Psychol.

Psychiatry

Psychiat.

Psychoanalytic Quarterly

Psychoanal. Quart.

Psychoanalytic Review

Psy choanal. Rev.

Psychological Bulletin

Psychol. Bull.

Psychological Monographs

Psychol.

Psychological Record

Psychol. Rec.

Psychological Review

Psychol. Rev.

Psychologische Rundschau

Psychol. Rundschau

Public Health Reports, Washington

Publ. Hlth. Rep.

Monog.

289
19

KEY TO REFERENCE ABBREVIATIONS
Public Opinion Quarterly

Publ. Opin. Quart.

Revue Internationale de Philosophic

Rev. Intern. Phil.

Sarah Lawrence Studies

Sarah Lawrence Stud.

School Musician

Sch. Mus.

School and Society

Sch.

Schweizerische Zeitschrijt Jiir Psychologie

Schweiz. Z. Psychol. Anwende.

und

ihre

Scientijic

and

Soc.

Anwendungen

Monthly

Sci.

Month.

Smith College Studies in Social Work

Smith Coll. Stud. Soc. Wk.

Social Forces

Soc. Forces

Teachers College Contributions to

Teach. Coll. Contrib. Educ.

Education
Teachers College Record

Teach. Coll. Rec.

Tohoku Psychologica Folia

Tohoku Psychol. Folia

University of Iowa Studies, Studies in

U. ojla. Stud. Child WelJ.

Child Welfare
University of Iowa Studies, Studies in the

U. of

la. Stud. Psjchol.

Mus.

Psychology of Music
University of Oregon Publication
Zeitschrijt Jiir Experimentelle

und

U. of Oregon Publ.

Zsch.J. Ang. Psjchol.

Angewandte Psychologie
Zeitschrijt JUr Laryngologie, Rhinologie, Otologic,

Zsch.J. Larjngol.

und ihre Grenzgebiete

Zeitschrijt Jiir Pddagogischc Psychologic
Zeitschrijt Jiir Psychologic

Zsch. Padg. Psychol.

und Physiologic Zsch.J. Psychol.

der Sinncsorgane
Zeitschrijt Jiir Psychotherapie

und

Medizinischc Psychologie
Zentralblatt Jiir die gesamte Neurologic

und Psychiatric
Zentralblatt Jiir Physiologic

290

Zsch.J. Psychother. Med.
Psychol.

Zentralbl. ges. Neurol, u.
Psychiat.

Zentralbl.

f.

Phjsiol.

Subject Index

(See also entries in Glossary beginning on page 280.)
ability, musical, see

beta learning, 202

musical ability

10—2

absolute pitch, 5^9—61, 78, 87, 231

biographies, musical,

accidentals, 20, 29

"blues" singers, 89—90

achievement, effects of music on, 264—270

bodily metabolism, music and, 2j^
body structures, and musical ability, 188—

music and, 264—26^
adaptation, in consonance perception, 48
adjectives, mood, 96-99, 102
Adler Music Appreciation Tests, 162
Aeolian Corporation, The, 69

activity,

aesthetics, experimental,

2

193

Boston Symphony Orchestra,
13^:.

African drums, rhythmic patterns

in,

iio,

program choices

133, 167—168

of,

Test, 161

73,

cadence

119

test,

164

age, and hearing ability,

Caprice Viennois, Kreisler, 89

Aliferis

cent, in scale nneasurement, 2^, 32

6^
Music Achievement Test, 229

American Musicological Society, 121, 123,
127—128, ly
aptitude tests, unstandardized,
see also

art, vs.

centitone, 2^
child prodigies, 198

2 3

1-2

3

j

musical aptitude tests

psychoneuroses, 209, 213

atonal music, 29,

children, musical ability in, 197

Chinese melodies,

finality in,

Chinese music, mistunings

4^—46

chordal structure, iio

attention and learning, principles of, ^7—58

chord-analysis test, 246

Ave Maria, Schubert, 9j

Bach family, 187
bagpipes, timbre of, 6^
baroque organ, 6^
basic abilities, 231-233
baton movements, £
Beach Standardization Music Tests, 226

Mamma, 90

beat, true, 4-5^
beat-instants,

and baton movements, ^-6

beauty, defined, 142
Seashore's rule of, 67

in,

43
120

chord recognition, 232
chromatic

Beale Street

130,

141

Bower Musical Moods

7—13

1

90

scale,

chromesthesia, 91-93
Cincinnati College of Music, 240
Clair de Lune, Debussy,
clarinet,

timbre

of,

96

64-65-

"classical music,"

layman's reaction to,

climatic

musical taste and,

cycles,

1

146-

147
college aptitudes, "critical levels" of,

1

color-tone linkage, 90-93

common

sense, unreliability of,

communication, desire

for,

3

84—85

291
19-2

SUBJECT INDEX
composers,

ranking

all-time,

of,

276-

277

modulation and, 32

biographical studies of, 210

born since 1870, ranking of, 276
eminence rankings of, 1 21-126, 276-

knowledge

of,

originality of,

i

expectancies, and tonalities, 45

factory work, music and,

211

of,

falling inflection,

Minor,

J. S.

Farnum Music Notation

Bach,

i

conductors, baton performances

^-6

264
and drum, 65

fatigue,
fife

fifth, in finality effect,

Fifth

9—10, 66

Fifth

interval and, 41

flute,

1

i

Dictionarj of Philosophy, 116

fusion, theory of, 48

dissonance, 47-48

dodecuple, 29, 32
dominant, 41, 76
Don Giovanni, Mozart, 262

Ganda

eight-string harp, 27

Gaston Test of Musicality, 161
German music, 141
Gernet Music Preference Test, 161

Doppelganger, Schubert, 107

Dorian mode, 86
bass,

64

French music, 141
French pitch,
frequency, law of, 57
frequency span, 38

diatonic scale, 18, 31

drone

36

flute tones, purity of,

decitone, 2^
deviation, artistic, 142

tests,

of thirds and sevenths, 89

flatting,

deafness, age and, 65, 78

Drake

41
Symphonj, Beethoven, 135, 148
Sjmphonj, Dvorak, 89

finality effects, 51

dance rhythms, unit groups in, 72
dance tempo, 69
Daphnis and Chloe, Ravel, 103

2

Test, 229

i

of,

consonance and dissonance, 47-^:1
creative process, 209—212, 215'

rationalization of,

267-268

42

male, 65

falsetto,

136-138

violins,

7—13

factor analysis, in musical ability, 180

210

space allocations for, in music histories,

D

in,

31-136

productive years

Cremona

Eroica Symphonj, Beethoven, 70, iio

experimental aesthetics, research

277

Concerto in

equal temperament, 25, 31, 87
as frame of reference, 28

Gestalt psychoanalysis, 209

243-244
76—79

grammar, musical language and, 85, 109
Gramophone Shop

Duo-Art player piano, 69

Music,

Guide

to

I

Encyclopedia

of Recorded

70

Recorded Music, A, 170

Ear Tests in Harmony, 229

Eastman

Conservatory

of

Music,

240
eminence, musical, and musical

taste,

11,

i

128
year of birth and, 127, 276-277
eminence rankings, 121 — 126, 276—277
changes in, 125—128

emphasis, law

of,

taste and,

epileptic attacks, 257

292

i

163, 181
high fidelity, and musical tastes, 150

57

Encyclopaedia Britannica, 171

enjoyment, musical

20-

harmonic sensitivity, test of, 145
harmonic series, 3 i
harmony, melody and, 75-77
in musical meaning, 99
stress on, in Western music, 21
Harvard Laboratory of Social Relations, o i
Hevner-Landsbury
discrimination
test,

i

29-1 31

Hindu

scale, 30

Home, Sweet Home, 95

SUBJECT INDEX
homophony, 77, 79
"hot jazz," 73-74

keyboard practice, 201
key-color associations, 91
see also color-tone linkage

Hutchinson Music Tests, 228
"hymnometer," 232

key-effects,

alleged,

and music language,

86-87
imagery, and musical abihty, 19^—196
and musical training, 204, 214
industrial music,

267-269

keyless music,

see

atonal music

keynote, 76, 103
defined, 41

inflection, falling, in finality effects, 42

kinesthetic imagery, 204

inhibition, retroactive, 203

Klangfarbe, 90

"instinctive" rhythm,

4

intelligence, and musical ability,
intensity,

183-184

and rhythm, 70—71
and Musi-

International Cyclopedia of Music

127
36-ji
apparent pitch of, 38-40
consonance and dissonance
cians,

interval,

in,

47-50

Knuth Achievement Tests in Music, 228
Kwalwasser Music Information and Appreciation Test, 165
Kwalwasser Music Talent Tests, 242
Kwalwasser-Dykema Test of Melodic Taste,
131, 145, 159-160, 240-242
Kwalwasser-Ruch Test of Music Accom-

plishment, 227

distinctive quality of, 36
falling inflection in,
finality effects

major-minor

language aspects of music, 84—1

42

and, 41

adjective

effects and,

i

major and minor scales, 88-89
music as language of emotions,

3

232

of, 37,

simultaneous, 47-50
size of,

and

finality effects,

stability of,

tonality and,

42

1

45-46

interval change, 32

learning, attention and,

interval distortion, 26
interval preference, loudness and, 62

57—58

function of, 58
musical, 199—203
learning efficiency, 200

44

interval tolerance, 36

L'Histoire du Soldat, Stravinsky, 103

interval uniqueness, 50

Liberia,

introvert-extrovert types,

Iowa, University of,

1

2

composers, 183
music, 148

i

3

5

melody and, 61—64
unit of, 63-64

rhythm and, 71, 73-74

loudness control, in musical instruments,

JuUiard School of Music, 234
just intonation, scale of, 21, 3

culture in, 107

loudness, 17, 89
and interval preference, 62

Javanese scale, 25—26
jazz,

Loma Negro

Lipps-Meyer law, 42, 44
listening power, test of, 161
London Philharmonic Orchestra,

94

I.Q., of great
Italian

93—

95
music as universal language, 106-108
psychoanalytic symbolism in, 08-1 10
"large" and "small," vs. major-minor, 40

26-27

interval resolutions,

2

1

95-99

color-tone linkage, 90—93
expression of tensions and, 102-106

and musical meaning, 10
recognition

for classifying,

alleged key effects, 86-87

40

melody hunting and, 40
preferences for,

lists

1

,

43

62-63
loudness losses, age and, 65-66

Keston Music Preference Test, 1 6
Keston Music Recognition Test, 161

Lundin Tests, 245—246
Lydian mode, 86

293

SUBJECT INDEX
"magnet tone," 41
major chord, three positions of, 8 8
major-minor effect, 40, £i, 109
major-minor modes, 88-90
major mode, "happy" nature of, 89—90,
major scale, 29
major sixth, stability of, 36
marijuana,

minor seventh, dissonance

and

"feminine"

1

10

need
Mohler

23—24

for,

Scales for

Measuring Judgment of

Orchestral Music, 162

composers,

monotones, human, 3-4
mood-adjectives, 96—99, 102—105

mood

108-109
meaning, in musical connmunication, 8^-86
mean-tone temperament, 24
Measure of Tonal Memory, £j

measurement of musical ability, 226-249
Drake tests, 243-244
Kwalwasser-Dykema tests, 240—242
Kwalwasser Music Talent Tests, 242-243
Lundin tests, 245-246
Seashore aptitude tests, 235-240
Strong Vocational Interest Test, 247
Tilson-Gretsch Test, 237

music, 94
Moravian music, 30
motor activity, rhythm and, 71
motor skills, and musical ability, 185
music, adjectives for classifying, 95-99
applications of, to therapy and industry,

254-270
chromesthetic behavior

91—93

in,

"classical" vs. "popular,"

1

144—146

cultural derivations,

distracting effects, 266
early history of,

231-23^
verbal and nonverbal skills, 226-231
Whistler-Thorpe test, 244-245
Wing standardized tests, 246-247
unstandardized aptitude

tests,

Measures of Consonance, 130
melodic taste, test of, 131, 145, 160
melody, 55-79

46
on achievement, 264-270
factor analysis in, 99-102
effect of,

grammar

of, 85
Grecian modes, 86

language aspects

of,

84-1

1

2

language of emotions, 93-95
love of, 143-144
as

mathematical

defined, ££
harmony and,

as

75—77
loudness and, 61—64

"meaning"

"microscopic," ^6
noise and, 67—69

psychological approach to, 1-14

art,

273

effects of, 86

psychoanalytic symbolism in, 108
physiological changes

pitch and, 58-61, 78

wrought by, 254-

258

rhythm and, 70—74
sonance and, 66—67
tempo and, 69—70
timbre and, 64—66
melody hunting, 40, 50

"purity" of, 273

memory,

sexual symbolism of, 108

reading

261
as socializing agent,

29,

In-

variables that give

88-89

"sadness" of, 90,

meaning

to,

99-102

musical ability, academic intelligence and,

ventory, 191

minor mode,

Personality

260—261

sound patterns in, 17
tone elements in, 17-18
as universal language, 106-108

millitone, 25

Multiphasic

205—206

260

and sense of achievement and prestige,

musical, 231, 244, 265-267

Minnesota

of,

rest and,

mental therapy, music in, 250-264
metabolism, effect of music on, 2^5
microtone, 29, 32

294

47

modality, in musical meaning, 99
modulation, in equal temperament, 32

3

"masculine"

of,

mistakes, "practising" of, 202

1

10

183
basic,

230-233

SUBJECT INDEX
training and, 147
whimsical or absolute nature

musical ability (cont.)

and body structures, 188-193

of,

117-

120

children and, 197
creativity and, 207—212

Music Journal, 247

178-179
development of, 196-199
and family lines, 187
Freudian view of, 194-1 9 j
generality of, 179-182
inheritance of, 184-187
imagery as source of, 19^-196
Jungian view of, 193-194
measurement of, see measurement of
defined,

musicologists, eminence ranks of, 126

"musicopoeia," 261

music

159-165
measurement of musical

tests,

see also

ability;

musical aptitude tests

music therapy, list of compositions favored
for, 202-263
present state

258-259

of,

Muzak Corporation, 269

musical ability
nature

178-21^

of,

and other

national anthem,

182

arts,

"natural" scale,

methods and, 199-203
training problems, 204-206
musical aptitude tests, 11, 226-249
future of, 248-249
see also measurement of musical ability
training

musical capacities, innate,

i

taste,

boredom

culture in

Nelson Silent Reading Test, 267

New

York Herald Tribune,

73

i

New York Philharmonic Orchestra, 135,141
New York Symphony Orchestra, 141
D

Chopin, 73

Flat,

noise, defined, 68

nomenclature
of,

142-152

cultural derivation of, 119
1

of, 3

Loma Negro

Liberia,

melody and, 67—69

and, 173—174

and conditioners

defined,

see also

Nocturne in

auditory tests for, 15^9-16^

climatic cycles and, 146
criteria

Negroes, tonal sensitivity

Ninth Sjmphonj, Beethoven, 135

i

musical learning, 199-203

musical

3
i

Nazi music, 10— 1

racial characteristics and, 188

"musical ear,"

3

difficulties,

70

nonrepetitive work, music and, 268

nonverbal musical

skills, tests of,

230-231

16

eminence of composer and, 120—128
enjoyment and 1 2 9- 1 3 i

"obe-imeter," 232

fluctuations in,

octave pattern, 20

,

34
individual and group differences
1

in,

i

38-

octave, 18-20

opera, musical taste

in,

142

orchestral color, and musical meaning, 102

142

knowledge of composers and, 131-136
of, 158-174
nature of,
16-153
orchestral programs and, 167-168
paper-and-pencil tests for, 165-167

organum, 76—79

polling of, 167

Oriental

measures

1

Oregon Music Discrimination
"beating" effects in, 68
electric, 68

and record broadcasts, 168—169

music,

lack

Western tones

scholarly texts on, 170—173

overtone,

space allocated to composers and, 136—

Oxford Companion

138
state creation

of,

Pacific

test for, 131,

145

147

Tests, 163

organ, baroque, 65

Gas

3

of
in,

sensitivity

for

45

1

&

to

Music, 127

Electric

Company

broadcasts,

134, 168—170

^95

SUBJECT INDEX
past,

reverence for, 126-128

prime numbers,

Peabody Conservatory of Music, 233
pedals, proper use of, 63
Peer Gynt Suite, Grieg, 89
pentatonic scale, 29, 32
Philadelphia Orchestra, 130, 141

1

10

psychological research, causal function in,

9—10

Philharmonic pitch, 17
phon, loudness unit, 63—64
listing,

1 1

Providence Inventory Test in Music, 228
psychic residua, in musical ability, 193
psychoanalytic symbolism, music and, 108-

Pastoral Symphony, Beethoven, 87, iio

phonograph

scale of, 22

taboo against, in music ratios,

pastoral music, 87

descriptive function in, 7—8
forecasting function, 7, 10— ii

musical taste and, 1^0—

Psychology oj a Musical Prodigy, 232

psychomusical investigators, limitations
physical therapy, music in, 2^9
see also

music therapy

psychoneurosis,

physiological changes, music and, 25-4—258

phonograph records, music

tests and,

1

59-

pure tones, 5, 78
Pythagorean scale, 21, 26, 31

piano lessons, beginning age for, 197
piano techniques, ability in, 185

quale, distinctive, of intervals, 36

quarter-note, tyranny of, 79
quarter-tone organ, 232

pitch, absolute, see absolute pitch

of intervals, 38

and major-minor modes, 89
and musical meaning, 100

quarter-tone scale, 30, 32

periodic changes

racial assessments, in

in, 8

Philharmonic, 17

musical ability, 188-

189
racism, musical, 120

233

pitch-deafness, 257

radio listening, musical tastes and,

pitch discrimination, 27
see also absolute pitch

range, and musical meaning,

pitch level,

polyrhythm

recordings, broadcasting of, 168

record

in, 6

studies,

research,

psychological,

resolution, of intervals,

composers

of,

psychological

44

1

resonance areas, in instruments, 66
"restless" or unresolved effects,

2," in finality effects,

keyboard, mass

vs.

43

distributed,

retroactive inhibition, 203

rhythm, defined, 70

precocity, 211, 215

melody and, 70-74
motor activity and,

primacy, law

"subjective," 73

201

of,

see

see also finality effects

i

positive pitch, 59
practice,

169-170

research

25
"popular music," layman's reaction to,

"power of

listings,

rehearing, and musical taste, 149
repetitive work, music and, 267

75

polyrhythms, 63-64, 79
polytonal music, 29, 46
classics,

57

51

of, 57
Record Book, The, 170

polymodality, 46
polyphony, 77

popular

1

recency, law

player piano, 69, 162

in

49-1
00-101

measurement, units of, 25
reading achievement, music and, 265—267

pitch wobble, 8

loudness differences

1

ratio

melody and, 58-61

pitch sensitivity, training and, 186

296

209, 213

vs. art,

psychotic, music therapy for, 259, 262
pulse rate, music beat and, 1 19

piano, loudness control in, 62-63

162

relative,

of,

6-7

71

44

SUBJECT INDEX
rhythm (cont.)
tempo and, 79

Stanford University Psychological Laboratory, 58-59, 96, 130, 244,

unit groups in, 72

rhythm discrimination, 232, 241-244
rhythmic ability, forecasting
"rhythmometer," 232, 234
rhythm preferences, 14^

Roth Quartet,
Rubin-Rabson

5
study,

of,

Strong Vocational Interest Test, 247
test, 227
subdominant, in finality effects, 41

243

Strouse music

sublimation, in musical ability, 195
in D Minor, Franck, 96

199-201

Symphony

symphony
minor

sadness, and

scale,

88—90

130

173
San Francisco City College, 129

synesthesia, 9

scale, diatonic, see diatonic scale

taste, musical, see

tempering, of intervals, 20

pentatonic, 29

psychological considerations of,

i

3

2 2

18—20

Power

in Music,

161

Seashore battery of music measures, 193

23^-237
239-240
Seashore Measures of Musical Talent, 236
1939 edition, 237—240
original,

validity of,

semitone, 25, 30
semitone steps, 20

variation in, 9
tensions, expression of,

Test of Musical Taste, 165
see also musical taste, measures of
therapy, music applications to, 254—270

music therapy

see also

237

sexual symbolism, of music, 108

Siamese chord, 89
Siamese scale, 25—26

timbre, 17, 78, 102
color and, 90
defined,

64
melody and, 64-66
timbre discrimination, 231
time signatures, "instinctive" rhythm

Siegfried motive, 106

205

as criterion

of consonance, 47

sonance, 78
melody and, 66

in,

4

tonal abilities, 185

musical ability

see also

tonal imagery, test for,

1

tonal impurities, 78
of,

64

physiological

tonal interval,

experiments

tonality,

see

interval

45—46

in atonal music,

by, 25^

space allotments, for composers, 136—138,

tonal
tonal

172
spans, absolute size of,

102—106

Test of Attitude toward Music, 165
Test for Musical Concepts, 163

Sense of Consonance Test, 49

Union,

of

Tilson-Gretsch Test for Musical Aptitude,

Seventh Symphony, Beethoven, iio

Soviet

meaning

melody and, 69—70
rhythm and, 79
tempo preferences, 145

Schultz Test of Listening

song "plugging," 149
soprano voices, purity

in

and major and minor modes, 89

Schelowo, Bloch, 103

smoothness,

of,

music, 99

types of, 17-32

sight reading,

importance

tempo,

scale changes, tolerance for, 32

scales, ancient,

musical taste

temperament, mean-tone, 24

2

scale of nature,

composer preferences

players,

of,

San Francisco Chronicle,

of just intonation,

264-265

stimulus configuration, 58
Stradivarius violin, 10

1

46

memory, 233
movement, measurement

test of,

of,

160

131

297

SUBJECT INDEX

of,

variables,

tone-word method, 61

37-38
and voice,

defined,
violin

MacDowell, 73
general problems
199-203
special problems of, 204—206
tremolo, defined, 37—38
trill, defined, 37-38
Tristan and Isolde, Wagner, 107
methods,

tritones, identification of, 37

4—^
see

pitch

typing, music and,

26^-267

5,

8

of well-known singers, 8
vibrato rate,

Tragic Sonata,

true pitch,

and musical meaning, 99—102

vibrato, 67, 78, 142

Torgerson-Fahnestock Music Test, 227
tragic music, 87

true beat,

toward,

verbal knowledge, tests of, 226-229

1

tonic, defined, 41

training

tendency

39

tones, duration of, 17

loudness

movement,

upward

tonal pattern, orientation of, 39

tone, pure vs. impure, g
tone color, 1 1 o

i

of,

Wagnerian symbolism, io6
war music, 148
weather, and musical

taste, 146—147
Western music, equal temperament in, 2j
stress on harmony in, 21

Whistler-Thorpe

Musical

244-245
whole-tone scale,

29, 32

Wing

Ability

Test,

Standardized Tests of Musical

Intelli-

gence, 246—247

unison singing, 75-76
University of Minnesota,

worker morale, music and, 268-270
i

30
Xerxes,

298

Handel, 89

Name

Adler, A.,

M.

Adler,

Bauer, E., 218

219

14,

2

Baumgarten,

17

J.,

Agnew, M., 220
Ainlay, G. W., 271
Aizawa, M.,

£g

i

Alchin, 40, ^2
Aldrich, C. K., 14

Alexander,
Aliferis, J.,

C,

1

1

Sir

109,

87,

123,

124,

30,

132,

260, 271

Bunch, C. C., 82

29,

1

Benham,

Areola, Pepito, 231

Berlin, Irving, i2j, 262

Aristoxenus, 20

Berlioz, Hector, 141, 196

Arnheim, R., 11^

Bienstock,
Jr.,

Berg,

271,

262
Atwell,

Avent,

E., 208,

S.

J.

E., 249

F.,

197,

I

5-6,

24,

I

W.

Campbell,

2^0

V.

D.,

80,

37, 141

,

Barlow,

J.,

6j, 109,

Bjorksten,

Case, Anna, 95

26,

Block, H. A., 83, 103

I

29,

148, 168, 169,
81

W.

J.,

211, 225-

T., ^, 14,

82, 204, 223

Bartok, Bela, 122, 263

Cattell,

Bodkin, M., 220

J.

McKeen,

Cattell, R. B., 162,

Bouvvsma, O. K., 113

Cazden, N., 49, 54
Chaliapin, Feodor, 8

Bower,

L. B., 175^, 181, 216,

122, 123,
I

3°,

169

I

I

24,

33-" 34,

171,

177

Borodin, Alexander, 262

220
Brahms, Johannes,

272

Bartholomew,

G., 112

Carmichael, Hoagy, 125
Caruso, Enrico, 8

1

171, 183, 262, 263

Bachem, A., 61,
Bahle, J., 224

I.

Capurso, A., 113, 217
Cardinell, R. L., 272

Bizet, Georges, 262

1

130- 132, 133-134, 13?,
I

W. H., 249
Buxtehude, Dietrich, 16

Butterfield,

217,

113

Bach, Johann Sebastian,
119, 123,

T., 25-0

Burton, R. V., 219

Birkhoff, G. D., 79
16, 20, 49,

S.

Burt, Cyril, 80

221

Bingham,

220

Burns,

224

A., 11^

I.

Billroth, T., 233,
S.,

Brown, L. M., 262, 271
Brown, M. E., 2j2
Brown, R. W., 114, 197,

182, 262, 263

1

133-134, 13s, 137, 141,

j,

Brighouse, G., 2j6, 270

148, 1^4, 168, 169, 171

26,

Anderson, Jean C, 17^
Antheil, George, 263
Antrim, D. K., 216

Arrington, G. E.,

56,

Brelet, G., 83

221, 222, 223
Bruckner, Anton, 117, 263
Bugg, E. G., J4

1

108,

M., 81

Britten, Benjamin, 122

G., 80

J.

Thomas, 70
Beethoven, Ludwig van,
70,

M.,

L.

Beebe-Center,

249
I.

Brammer,

Breithaupt, R., 218

Allen, R. D., 249

Altshuler,

Braine, R., 218

216

F.,

Beach, F. A., 249
Bean, K. L., 205-, 220, 223
Becker, H. S., 208, 224

Beecham,

216

Index

Chandler, A. R.,
^6,
I

26,

HI,

109,
I

29,

168,

1

1

17^

i'

Charney, D., 2^2
Cheslock, L., I
5^

Chien Lohtze, 29
Chinn, H. A., 50
i

299

NAME INDEX
Chopin,
109,

Frederic,
I

24,

72,

73,

26, 129, 132,

I

262

M., 219

East, E.

Thomas

Edison,

Christiansen, H., 217

Edmunds,

Cochran, M., 197, 221
Colby, M. G., 221
Connette, E., 217
Copland, Aaron, 122, 262

Eggen,

Corso,

Ehrsam, E., 216

J. F.,

i^,

5^3

Frances, R., 80

Eberly, L. E., 222

E.

Franck, Cesar, 96, 263

A., 94,

i

13

M., 37, 52

J. B., 83, 1^7
Ehrenzweig, Anton, 209—210,

Frankenstein,

Alfred,

1^4, 173
Franklin, E., 53,

Courtis, S. A., 160, 17^

Einstein, Albert, 1^2

Cowan, M., 82

Eisenberg, P., ijo, 156

Friend, R. S., 217

Cowell, Henry, 68, 73, 196,
221, 224
Cowles, J. T., 107, 115^

Eitz, Karl, 81

Frischeisen-Kohler,

Cox, C, 216
Crews, A., 272
Critchley, M., 270

Dannenbaum,
Dashiell,

Debussy, Claude, 29, 96, 109,
123,1 26, I 29, I 30, I 39,
168, 2^6, 262-263

De

14

M. W.,

Fuller,

W., 113

Galton, Sir Francis, 19^

Gardner,

English, H. B., 115-

Gardner, P. A.,

Esman, A. H., 220

Garrison, K.

Gatewood,

R.,

35-,

34,

Gaw,

E. L., 9^, 112

E. A., 251, 236

Gei^er, T.,

114, 115, IJ2, 1^4, iss,

Geiringer, K.,

57,

I

j6
197, 221

Gaston, E., 174
Gaston, T., 15-6

Fahestock, E., 249
Fairbanks, G., 82

I

i

C,

^2, ^3, 81, 82, 83, 112,

Denny, M. R., 267, 272
Densmore, F., 270

148, 272

J. E.,

113

J.,

15^^

81

Engle, T. L., 271

Dennis, Wayne, 211, 22

Delay,

I.,

Galli-Curci, Amelita, 8, 179

L. E., j6, 80

Farnsworth, P.

Grazia, S., 114

2

Froschels, E., 197, 221

53

D. S., 2^6, 270
Elman, Mischa, 66
Elton,

1^6

J. F.,

220,

Ellis,

Emerson,

A., 2^j, 270

I.,

216,

181,

2(^o

Fredrich, F., 223
Freeburne, C. M., 272
Freud, Sigmund,
194—19^,

224
Ehrlich, S., 83

Elkus, A.

129,

76, 177, 2 19, 220,

Gernet,

S.

i

£
2

17

K., 175

Farnum,

Gershwin, George, i2j, 140,
15^4, 262

Fay, P. J., 2^2
Fay, R. V., 114

Gigli,

Fendrick, P., 266, 272

Gilbert, G. M., 217,2 19, 25-3

Doig, D., 221, 224

Fine, H., 2^^^,

Gildersleeve, G., 227, 249

Downey,

Fischer, E., 218

223, 2JI, 2J2, 25^3, 272
S. E., 248, 2^0

D'Indy, Vincent, 141
Diserens,

C. M.,

25^,

264,

270, 271

Disney, Walt,

1

J. E.,

1

i

£

£y

Drager, H. H., 34
Drake, R. M., 164, 176,

21^^,

202, 223
Dunlevy, E. C, 2^2
Dutton, C. E., 115^
Dvorak, Anton, 89
P.

W.,

264

17^, 2^3

O. W.,

Easeley, E., i^

300

15-4

8

Fleischer, K., 221

Gilman, B. J., 174
Gilman, L., 271

Fleischer, M., 272

Girard,

i

^7

J., 271
Glazunov, Aleksandr, 141

Fletcher, H., 81

Fokker, A. D., 3^
Foley,

J. P., Jr.,

1^6,

2

9, 15,

14^,

19

Folgmann, E. E., 130, 154
Forbes, H. B., 221
Forbes, H. S., 221
Ford, A., 2^2
Foster, Stephen, i2y, 129

Eagleson,

Beniamino,

Gilliland, A. R., 1^7

Fisher, R. L., 113,

253
Dreher, R. E., 112, 256, 270
Drexler, E. N., 217
Dunlap, K., ^2, ^4, 72, 83,

Dykema,

Giese, F., 14

Foster,

W.

J.,

Fraisse, P., 83

i

54

Gordon, Kate, 83
Gounod, Charles Francis, 196
Graf, Max, 209, 224
Graf, S., 218

Grandprey, M., 221
Granneberg, R., 154
Greene, P. C, 28, 34
Gregory the Great, Pope
33

Gretsch, Fred, 2^2

St.,

NAME INDEX
Grieg, Edvard, 89

Higginson,

Grimm, W., 216

Hildum, D.

Grimmett,

Hill,

O., 220

J.

Grofe, F., 262

Gros, R. R.,

I

W.

C,

Keston, M.

14

1

J. P., 80, 22^
Gundlach, R., 99-101, 107,

Hitler, Adolf, 10

114
Gunther,

Hollingworth, L.

Hodgson, Walter, 14

M.

Hollinshead,

^7, 17^,

S.,

M., 253
Knapp, G. E., i ^7
Knuth, W. E., 249
Koch, H., 217
Kock, W. E., i^
Kichmann, R., 221
Kleist,

Hindemith, Paul, 122

218

i

Keys, N., 218, 234, 2 ji
Khachaturian, Avram, 262

Hillbrand, E. K., 249
Hilton, R. A., 80

Guilford,

j6,

i

J.,

191, 219, 220

G., 82

Hillard, B., 263, 271

^^

Gross, B., 220, 224
Grunewald, M., 270

S.,

Kern, Jerome, 12^, 262

H., 11^

J.

216, 217

R., ig

W.,

Holmes,

J.

Haba, 30
Haecker, V., 217, 233, 2^0
Haggin, B. H., 170, 176

Homer,

8

Hall, D., 170

Himgerland, Helmut, 174
Hurley, J. S., 81
Husband, R. W., 264, 271

Kovacs,

Friedrich,

Hutchinson, H. E., 249
Hutchison, W. O., 224

126,

Huygens, Christiaan, 35

Kries, J. von, 233, 2^0
Krone, M. T., 250
Kruger, F., 47, ^3
Krugman, H. E., 1^7
Kwalwasser, J., 130, i6(^, 175,
176, 219, 249, 2JI, 253

Hall, J.
Haller,

C,

256, 272

M. W.,

Hampton,

221

P. J., 113

George

Handel,

72, 89,

137, 141

Handschin,

123,
,

124,

115

J., ^2, 5^3,

Harrington, R.

J.,

Kotick,

Koussevitzky, Serge, 70, 141

F.,

I

^^

Ingerham, D. W., 211, 22^
Irion, A. L., 202, 223

D.

B.,

Ives, Charles,

^4
263

O.

Jacobsen,

Hartmann, A., 220
Hattwick, M., i j, 217, 221,

Jancke, H., 224

236,

I

25-1

Joseph,

26,

I

I.,

206,

224,

Kretschmer, E., 218

137,

123,

30, 141, 168, 171,

183, 210

Jancke,

J.,

218, 234,

J.,

W.

i£2

S.,

Lavere,

W., 271

Jaques-Dalcroze, E., 74, 83
Jensen, M. B., 271

Lawshe, C. H.,

Jeritza, Maria, 8

Learned,

Jersild, A. T., 197, 217, 221

Lehman, C.
Lehman, H.

Jr.,

J.,

217
F., 211,

C,

Jesus Christ, 148

Heiliger, L., 217

Johnson, D. M., 270
Johnson, G. B., 14, 218

Leibold, R., 221

Jung, Carl, 193-194, 214

Lenz, F., 218

5-4,

63, 80,

81, 89, 112
Hell, F. J., 222

Helmholtz, H. L.

252

Lazarfeld, P. F., 15^6

Heifetz, Jascha, 66, 199

Heinlein, C. P.,

224

Lastrucci, C. L., 208,

209, 224

25^1

J. L., 163

Lannert, V., 205, 223
Larson,

2^1

249

201, 222

S.,

Landsburg,

Harris, J. D., 252
C, 81

L., 229,

Kreisler, Fritz, 89, 199

Lamp, C.

81

Hart, H.

Haydn,

M.

Howells, T. H., 92, 113

Howes,

Irvine,

156

L.,

81

Kolodin, Irving, 170

168, 171, 183

Hardcastle, Arthur, 74
Harrell, T. W., 155

Harriman, P.

Kohler,

A., 241, 25-3

216

225-

Leiter, R. A., 114

Lewis, D., IS, 80, 83, 222,
F., 34, 47,

Henderson, M. T., 83, 266,
272
Henkin, R. I., 102, 114, 270
Hevner, K., 96, 97-99, 102,
103, 112, 114, 134, 141,
i^^, 163, 16^

Kaiser, L.,

5^2,

112

2^2

Kant, Immanuel, j8
Kaplan, I., 272

Licht, S. H., 271

Kaplan, M., i££

Lincoln, Abraham, 148

Karlin, J. E., 181, 216
Karwoski, T., 113, 114

Linder, F. E.,

Kelley,

N. H., 82

Kelly, E. L., 92, 113

Lichte,

W.

H., i^j

i

^

Lipps, T., 42, 48, S2, S4.
Liszt, Franz, 124, 126, 141,

137, 2^6, 262

301

NAME INDEX
Ortmann, O.,

Loar, L., 209, 224

Miller, R. S.,

London, I. D., 270
Lowenstein, O., 2^6, 270
Lowery, H., 164, 176, 205^,

Milstein, Nathan, 66

80,

Mjon,

I

223, 233, 2^0, 2^1

i

<^

217, 233, 2^0

J.,

Montani, A., 108, 115^
Monteverdi, Claudio, 126

Lund, M., 14, 69, 83
Lundin, R. W., ^4, 72, 83,

Montgomery, H. C,
Moon, P., 34

216, 253
Lusby, W. S., 81

Moore, H. T., 48,
Moorhead, G.

McAllester, D. P., ^3
McCauley, C. J., 249

82

25^3

McNemar, Q., 176

Palestrina,

Marx, Karl,

1

Pear, T. H., 231, 25^0

Peatman,

J. G., i j6
Pepinsky, A., 114

183, i8j, 196, 262

Pickford,

H.,

J.

1

34-1

35-,

141, ^55, 176
Mull, Helen K., 81, 95, 113,

L.,

J.

83,

71,

Pinto,
Piatt,

I.

R.W., 148,1^6, 224
M., 156, lyg, 220

W., 197, 221
W., 197, 221

Plotkin,

Plotkin, E. G., 156

32,

3^, ^3,
216, 222,

119,

Pond, D., 217
Ponselle, Rosa, 8

W., 34

Poole, H.

2^1

Myers, C.

S.,

54

J.,

Podolsky, E., 270, 271, 272

T., 17 j

Mursell,

180, 21 j
10

148, 171

Paperte, F., 271

Petran, L. A., 80

149, ij4, IS7

S.,

W., 249

A., 216

Peterson,

Munro,

Marill, G., 1^4.

W.

141, 148, 168, 169, 171

Maltzew, C. V., 37, 52
Manor, H. C, 239, 2^2
Manzer, C. W., 180, 215^
Marowitz,

Owens,

123,1 24, I 26, I 29, I 30,
132, 133-134, ns, 137,

Mueller,

iji, 15-7

Porter, Cole,

i

C,

Pratt, C.

Nash, D.,

Masson, D. L, 113
Mendelssohn, Felix, 117, 126,
132,1 37, 141, 171, 262

Nelson, H. M., 80

Pressey, L.

Nestele, A., 222

Preston, D., 82

Nettel, R., 272

Prokofiev,

Nevin, Ethelbert, 263
New, D. M., 80

263
Ptolemy,

Newton,

Pyle,

Metfessel, M., 15, 66, 82

Meyer, Max,

14,

4,

20,

33,

34, 42, 52, 80, 8i, 88,

224

93, 107, III, 113

90

Sir Isaac,

Nickerson,

J. F.,

Mezzrow, M., 14

Nielsen,

T., 2^1

Michaeles, A. F., 154
Michel, A., 220

Nyiregyhazi, Erwin, 185, 232

Michelman,

J.,

Oakes,

Middleton,

W.

112, 119,

Si, 232, 2^0

I

i

5

C., 252, 271

J.

W.

28, 34

Milhaud, Darius, 46, j
Miller, R. E., 82

Omwake,

Miles,

302

C.

J.

C.,

191,

R., 270

219,

L., 91, 112,

228, 249

Serge,

2

122,

140,

i

H., 217

Pythagoras, 18, 28

Quinan,

C,

190, 219
Sergei,

122,

140, 141, 174

J., 236—237, 2^2
Odbert, H. S., 114
Offenbach, Jacques, 262
Ogden, R. M., 48, ^4
Olson, H. F., I £j

Miles,

W.

C,

Rachmaninov,

F., 81

O'Brien, C. C., 222

O'Connor,

Mikol, B., 267, 272

2^

26, 34, ^4, 80,

Mascagni, Pietro, 262

15-,

iji,

Paderewski, Ignace Jan, 140
Giovanni,
126,

217

S., 216
Mosher, R. M., 249
Mosonyi, D., 108, 115
Mozart, Wolfgang Amadeus,

Madison, T. H., 234, 2ji
Mahler, Gustav, 117, 141
Mainwaring, J., 220, 222,

113,

sj, 205-, 223, 250

Otterstein, A.
ij6,

5^4,

Morrow, R.

McGehee, W., 272
McGeoch, J. A., 202, 223

82,

Osborne, E., 218
Osgood, C. E., 114
Ossipowa, S., 220

Morini, Erika, 66

MacDowell, Edward, 73
McElroy, W. A., 112

McGinnis, E. M., 251
McLeish, J., 180, 2ij,

E.,

37, ^2, ^7, 62,

81,

115

Racker, H., 220
Raff, Joseph, 92

Ramon, K. M., 14
Rashevsky, N., 1^3
Ravel, Maurice, 103,
133-134, 140, 263

122,

NAME INDEX
Razran, G. H.

Regner,

Rethberg, Elizabeth,
Revesz,

Schuessler, K. P., 146,

1^7

S.,

N., 15

S.

G.,

8

216,

82,

224,

Rife,

D. C., 216

Rigg,

M.

I

Rimsky-Korsakov,

Nicolai,

115^,

253

Rittershaus, E., 218

Robinson, W., 148, i j6
Rodgers, Richard, 12^
Roe, A., 2^0
Rogge, G. O., 103, 10^, 114
Romberg, Sigmund, 12^

C,

34

Rosenthal, A., 219
Rossini,

Ernestine,

16
1

130,

,

171,

177
Rothschild, D. A., 15
Rubin-Rabson, G., 157, 199-

Stone,

W., 2ji

Seashore, Carl E.,

262

2, 8 n.,

142, 143, ^SS, 1^9, I7S,
180, 190, 193, 198, 2 17,

222,

219,

23J,

240,

243, 244, 2J2
Seashore, H. G., i j

R. H.,

i6j,

217,

220, 224, 232, 23J, 2JI

Semenoff, B., 174, 176
Shaw, George Bernard, 10

Shimp,

112

B.,

14,

Dmitri,

109,

140

Sibelius, Jan,

J.,

148,

Strauss,

Stravinsky,

Igor,

Strong, E. K., Jr., 216, 2^3
Strouse, C. E., 249

Stumpf, C., 33, 38, 39, 48,
52, Si, 216, 231, 2JO

Suchman, E. A., 149, i j6
Sugarman, P., 263, 271
Sullivan, Sir Arthur,

66
Szucharewa, G., 220
Szigeti, Joseph,

133-134, 141,

168, 169, 263

M.

L., 190,

2

19

Talley, Marion, 8

Simon, B., 271
Simonton, T. E., 27

Tangeman, R.

Sjostrom, L.,

Taylor,

i

j

Small, A.,

iji,

216,

1^7,
2

113,

164,

176,

JO, 271, 272

Schonberg, Arnold, 46, 122
Scholes, P. A., I J, 113
Schramek, R., 224

Schrammel, H.
Schubert,

E.,

9^,

109, 123, 124,
130,

132,

I

26,

137,

168, 169, 262

W., 37, j4
Smith, G. H., 236, 2ji
Smith, H. B., 2ji

J.

H., 218

107,
I

29,

148,

Smith, H. R., 2j2
Smith, M. E., j2

Soibelman, D., 271
Sopchak, A. L., 113
Soper,
Sousa,

W.,
J. P.,

i

54

Peter

I.,

124,

227, 249
89,

I

US,

137,
1^4,
168, 169, 262

163,

Terry, C., 217

Thompson, A.

Smith, H. C., 272
Smith, H. D., 250

Snyder, L. H., 216

249

Franz,

177

126, 129, 132, 133-134,

J

Smith, P.
9^,

15^,

I

S.,

Taylor, E. M., 2^3

Tchaikovsky,

M.,

12 j

Sunderman, L. P., 218
Sward, Keith, 218

Slonimsky, N., 217

Schoen,

122,

103,

Streep, R. L., 218

Saunders, D. R., 162,
Scheinfeld, A., 217

129,

133, 140, 148

Taylor, L. E.,

i ^^

154

109,

•32, 133-134, 140, 168

Skinner, L., 83

Saunders, P. A., 9,

56

Richard,

Salisbury, F. S., 250, i£i, 25^2
175^

I

Strauss, Johann, i2j,

49,
52, 54, S7, 61, 67, 80,
81, 82, 83, 112, 130,

Sikes,

Saetveit, J. G., 80, 222, 2^2
Camille,
Saint-Saens,
141,

Stempel, G. H., 177

Scriabin, Aleksandr, 29, 91

1

Runes, Dagobert, 116, 1^3
Rupp, H., 233, 2 JO

224
2jo

I.,

Stevens, S. J., 33
Stone, C. L., 147, ij6

Shostakovitch,

201, 203, 222, 223

Ruck, G., 249

M.

Stelzer, T. G.,

Schwartz, H. D., 219
Schweisheimer, W., 218

Seashore,

Gioacchino,

1 1

j6

i

2J2
Stein,

Scripture, E.

29

Rittenhouse, C. H.,

Roncalio, A.

Stanton, H. M., 11, 16, 217,

132, 137, 141; 169, 210

112

J,

Riker, B. L., 80

I

Stanton, P.,

Schuman,W., 140

Schumann, Robert,

G., 10,

91, 109,

Staffelbach, E. H., 143, ijj

8,

Riggs, L. A., 113

Squires, P. C., 219, 220, 224

Schultz, E. J., 17

Schumann-Heink,

232, 250
Richardson, E. G., ij

j6

i

Schullian, D. M., 271, 272

29

S., 54
Thorpe, L. P., 2^3
Thurstone, L. L., 80, i6j
Thus, S., 113
Tibbett, Lawrence, 8

Tiffin, J., 12, 16

Tilly, C. R., 108,
Tilly,

M.,

Tilson, J.

270

2J7
M., 252
1 1

J,

303

NAME INDEX
Tinker,

M.

Voegelin, C. P., 3^,

A., 1^4.

Tolmie, J. R., 1 ^
Torgerson, T. L., 229, 249
Toscanini, Arturo, 70
Totenberg, Roman, 66
Trabue, M. R., 175

Trembley, J. C,
Tully, M., 249

11 j

81

^^3,

i

Wachsmann, K. P., 27
Wagner, A. H., ^
Wagner, Richard, 109,

Whittaker,

124,

Wakeham,

126,

iii,
130,

G., 202, 223

Updegraff, R., 43, ^3, 217

Walker, E., 198, 221
Washburn, M. P., 1^7
Washington, George, 148

Vaerting, M., 190, 219

Waterman,

W.,

Valentine, C.

112,1

Van
Van

^£, 22

39, j2, 88,

Briessen, M., 222

Vance, T. F., 221
Van de Wall, W., 271
Van Nuys, K., 206, 223

I

Williams,

Ralph,

22

Vernon, L.,
Vernon, P.
ids,

'

1

££

E.,

76,

13,
I

224
Verneer, E. M.,

304

16,

1^7,

80, 21^, 219,

i

s7

J.

W.

G., 224

83

G., 2^0

i j6
Williams, G. D., 156
Williams, H. M., 217, 221,

2JI

Willmann, R. R., 11^
Wilson, M. E., 222
Wing, H. D., 176, 180,

215-,

2^3

Wolfe, B., 14, 191
Wolfe, L. S., 191 219
Wolner, M., 217
,

Wood, C. P., 249
Wood, W. P., 252
Wright,

P. A.,

2^2

Wunderlich, H., 53, 80
Wyatt, R. P., 217, 2^1

Wells, A., 221

Varro, M., 221

Vaughan

Watkins,

W. C,

Watson, K. B., 112
Watt, H. J., 39, ^2
Weaver, H. E., 83, 206, 223
Weber, Carl Maria von, 19 j
Wedell, C. H., 81

I

Alstyne, D., 218

T., 253

Wiebe, G., 149,

i

143, 1^4, 168, 169, 171

Ullman, M., 2oj, 223

271

S.,

Whittaker, A. H., 208, 218

123,

^6

M.

Whitley,

133-134, 135, 137, 141,

Tyler, L. E., 147,

Whiting, H.

Voss, G., 217

Wendelin, A., 271
Werner, E. M., 3o, 221, 272
Wheeler, R. H., 146, 156
Wheelwright, L. P., 223
Whipple, G. M., 217
Whistler, H. S., 2^3
White, R. K., 216

Yasser, J., 34
Young, M., i6j, 176

Whitely, P. L., 265, 272

Zipf, G. K., 176

Zaworski, T., 221
Zener, K. E., 42, j2
Ziehen, T., 217, 233, 2jo
Zigler,

M.

J.,

113

DATE DUE
1

1

GAYLORD

PRINTED

IN U.S.A.

WELLESLEY COLLEGE LIBRARY

3 5002 03353 0705

Music ML 3830
Farnsworth,
1899-1978.

.

F215

Paul

R.

The social psychology of
music.
WELLESLEY COLLEGE LIBRARY

Sponsor Documents

Or use your account on DocShare.tips

Hide

Forgot your password?

Or register your new account on DocShare.tips

Hide

Lost your password? Please enter your email address. You will receive a link to create a new password.

Back to log-in

Close